Skip to main content

[]

Intended for healthcare professionals
Skip to main content
Restricted access
Research article
First published online September 21, 2015

What Is the Role of Manual Preference in Hand-Digit Mapping During Finger Counting? A Study in a Large Sample of Right- and Left-Handers

Abstract

The goal of the present study was to test whether there is a relationship between manual preference and hand-digit mapping in 369 French adults with similar numbers of right- and left-handers. Manual laterality was evaluated with the finger tapping test to evaluate hand motor asymmetry, and the Edinburgh handedness inventory was used to assess manual preference strength (MPS) and direction. Participants were asked to spontaneously “count on their fingers from 1 to 10” without indications concerning the hand(s) to be used. The results indicated that both MPS and hand motor asymmetry affect the hand-starting preference for counting. Left-handers with a strong left-hand preference (sLH) or left-hand motor asymmetry largely started to count with their left hand (left-starter), while right-handers with a strong right-hand preference (sRH) or right-hand motor asymmetry largely started to count with their right hand (right-starter). Notably, individuals with weak MPS did not show a hand-starting preference. These findings demonstrated that manual laterality contributes to finger counting directionality. Lastly, the results showed a higher proportion of sLH left-starter individuals compared with sRH right-starters, indicating an asymmetric bias of MPS on hand-starting preference. We hypothesize that the higher proportion of sLH left-starters could be explained by the congruence between left-to-right hand-digit mapping and left-to-right mental number line representation that has been largely reported in the literature. Taken together, these results indicate that finger-counting habits integrate biological and cultural information.

Get full access to this article

View all access and purchase options for this article.

References

Andres M., Davare M., Pesenti M., Olivier E., Seron X. (2004) Number magnitude and grip aperture interaction. Neuroreport 15: 2773–2777.
Andres M., Michaux N., Pesenti M. (2012) Common substrate for mental arithmetic and finger representation in the parietal cortex. NeuroImage 62: 1520–1528. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.047.
Andres M., Olivier E., Badets A. (2008) Actions, words, and numbers: A motor contribution to semantic processing? Current Directions in Psychological Science 17: 313–317. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00597.x.
Ardila A. (2010) On the evolution of calculation abilities. Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience 2. doi:10.3389/fnevo.2010.00007.
Badets A., Andres M., Di Luca S., Pesenti M. (2007) Number magnitude potentiates action judgements. Experimental Brain Research 180: 525–534. doi:10.1007/s00221-007-0870-y.
Badets A., Pesenti M., Olivier E. (2010) Response-effect compatibility of finger-numeral configurations in arithmetical context. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 63: 16–22. doi:10.1080/17470210903134385.
Barsalou L. W. (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 577–609.
Barsalou L. W., Kyle Simmons W., Barbey A. K., Wilson C. D. (2003) Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 84–91.
Butterworth B. (1999) The mathematical brain, London, England: MacMillan.
Dehaene S., Bossini S., Giraux P. (1993) The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 122: 371–396.
Dehaene S., Tzourio N., Frak V., Raynaud L., Cohen L., Mehler J., Mazoyer B. (1996) Cerebral activations during number multiplication and comparison: A PET study. Neuropsychologia 34: 1097–1106.
Di Luca S., Granà A., Semenza C., Seron X., Pesenti M. (2006) Finger-digit compatibility in arabic numeral processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 59: 1648–1663. doi:10.1080/17470210500256839.
Di Luca S., Pesenti M. (2008) Masked priming effect with canonical finger numeral configurations. Experimental Brain Research 185: 27–39.
Fabbri M. (2013) Finger counting habits and spatial-numerical association in horizontal and vertical orientations. Journal of Cognition and Culture 13: 95–110.
Fayol M., Barrouillet P., Marinthe C. (1998) Predicting arithmetical achievement from neuro-psychological performance: A longitudinal study. Cognition 68: B63–B70.
Fias W., Fischer M. (2005) Spatial representation of numbers. In: Campbell J. I. D. (ed.) Handbook of mathematical cognition, New York, NY: Psychology Press, pp. 43–54.
Fischer M. H. (2008) Finger counting habits modulate spatial-numerical associations. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 44: 386–392. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.004.
Fischer M. H., Brugger P. (2011) When digits help digits: Spatial-numerical associations point to finger counting as prime example of embodied cognition. Frontiers in Psychology 2: 260. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00260.
Fischer M. H., Mills R. A., Shaki S. (2010) How to cook a SNARC: Number placement in text rapidly changes spatial-numerical associations. Brain and Cognition 72: 333–336. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.010.
Fuson K. (1988) Children's counting and the concepts of number, New York, NY: Springer.
Gracia-Bafalluy M., Noël M. P. (2008) Does finger training increase young children's numerical performance? Cortex 44: 368–375. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.020.
Haken H., Kelso J. A. S., Bunz H. (1985) A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biological Cybernetics 51: 347–356.
Huber S., Klein E., Graf M., Nuerk H. C., Moeller K., Willmes K. (2014) Embodied markedness of parity? Examining handedness effects on parity judgments. Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-014-0626-9.
Ifrah G. (1981) The universal history of numbers: From prehistory to the invention of numbers, London, England: The Harvill Press.
Kaufmann L. (2008) Dyscalculia: Neuroscience and education. Educational Research: A Review for Teachers and All Concerned with Progress in Education 50: 163–175. doi:10.1080/00131880802082658.
Lindemann O., Alipour A., Fischer M. H. (2011) Finger counting habits in middle eastern and western individuals: An online survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42: 566–578.
Mazoyer B., Mellet E., Perchey G., Zago L., Crivello F., Jobard G., Tzourio-Mazoyer N. (2015) BIL&GIN: A neuroimaging, cognitive, behavioral, and genetic database for the study of human brain lateralization. NeuroImage. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.071.
Mazoyer B., Zago L., Jobard G., Crivello F., Joliot M., Perchey G., Tzourio-Mazoyer N. (2014) Gaussian mixture modeling of hemispheric lateralization for language in a large sample of healthy individuals balanced for handedness. PloS One 9: e101165. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0101165.
Moeller K., Martignon L., Wessolowski S., Engel J., Nuerk H. C. (2011) Effects of finger counting on numerical development – The opposing views of neurocognition and mathematics education. Frontiers in Psychology 2: 328. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00328.
Noël M. P. (2005) Finger gnosia: A predictor of numerical abilities in children? Child Neuropsychology 11: 413–430. doi:10.1080/09297040590951550.
Oldfield R. C. (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9: 97–113.
Pesenti M., Thioux M., Seron X., De Volder A. (2000) Neuroanatomical substrates of Arabic number processing, numerical comparison, and simple addition: A PET study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12: 461–479.
Previtali P., Rinaldi L., Girelli L. (2011) Nature or nurture in finger counting: A review on the determinants of the direction of number-finger mapping. Frontiers in Psychology 2: 363. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00363.
Sato M., Lalain M. (2008) On the relationship between handedness and hand-digit mapping in finger counting. Cortex 44: 393–399. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.005.
Sato M., Cattaneo L., Rizzolatti G., Gallese V. (2007) Numbers within our hands: Modulation of corticospinal excitability of hand muscles during numerical judgment. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19: 684–693. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.4.684.
Spijkers W., Heuer H., Kleinsorge T., van der Loo H. (1997) Preparation of bimanual movements with same and different amplitudes: Specification interference as revealed by reaction time. Acta Psychologica 96: 207–227.
Stanescu-Cosson R., Pinel P., van De Moortele P. F., Le Bihan D., Cohen L., Dehaene S. (2000) Understanding dissociations in dyscalculia: A brain imaging study of the impact of number size on the cerebral networks for exact and approximate calculation. Brain 123: 2240–2255.
Tschentscher N., Hauk O., Fischer M. H., Pulvermüller F. (2012) You can count on the motor cortex: Finger counting habits modulate motor cortex activation evoked by numbers. NeuroImage 59: 3139–3148. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.037.
Wasner M., Moeller K., Fischer M. H., Nuerk H. C. (2014) Aspects of situated cognition in embodied numerosity: The case of finger counting. Cognitive Processing 15: 317–328. doi:10.1007/s10339-014-0599-z.
Wood G., Nuerk H. C., Moeller K., Geppert B., Schnitker R., Weber J., Willmes K. (2008) All for one but not one for all: How multiple number representations are recruited in one numerical task. Brain Research 1187: 154–166. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2007.09.094.
Zago L., Pesenti M., Mellet E., Crivello F., Mazoyer B., Tzourio-Mazoyer N. (2001) Neural correlates of simple and complex mental calculation. NeuroImage 13: 314–327. doi:10.1006/nimg.2000.0697.
Zebian S. (2005) Linkages between number concepts, spatial thinking, and directionality of writing: The SNARC effect and the reverse SNARC effect in English and Arabic monoliterates, biliterates, and illiterate Arabic speakers. Journal of Cognition and Culture 5: 165–190.

Cite article

Cite article

Cite article

OR

Download to reference manager

If you have citation software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice

Share options

Share

Share this article

Share with email
Email Article Link
Share on social media

Share access to this article

Sharing links are not relevant where the article is open access and not available if you do not have a subscription.

For more information view the Sage Journals article sharing page.

Information, rights and permissions

Information

Published In

Article first published online: September 21, 2015
Issue published: January-February 2016

Keywords

  1. Handedness
  2. finger counting
  3. hand-starting preference

Rights and permissions

© The Author(s) 2015.
Request permissions for this article.
PubMed: 26562861

Authors

Affiliations

Laure Zago
Groupe d’Imagerie Neurofonctionnelle (GIN), Université de Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
Arnaud Badets
Groupe d’Imagerie Neurofonctionnelle (GIN), Université de Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France

Notes

Laure Zago, Groupe d’Imagerie Neurofonctionnelle (GIN), UMR 5296, CNRS, CEA Université de Bordeaux, F-33000, France. Email: [email protected]

Metrics and citations

Metrics

Journals metrics

This article was published in Perception.

View All Journal Metrics

Article usage*

Total views and downloads: 175

*Article usage tracking started in December 2016


Altmetric

See the impact this article is making through the number of times it’s been read, and the Altmetric Score.
Learn more about the Altmetric Scores



Articles citing this one

Receive email alerts when this article is cited

Web of Science: 3 view articles Opens in new tab

Crossref: 4

  1. Cultural variability in finger representations / Variabilidad cultural en las representaciones con dedos
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle Scholar
  2. Cultural similarities and specificities of finger counting and montring: Evidence from Amazon Tsimane' people
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle Scholar
  3. Space, time and number: common coding mechanisms and interactions between domains
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle Scholar
  4. Cardinal and ordinal aspects of finger-counting habits predict different individual differences in embodied numerosity
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle Scholar

Figures and tables

Figures & Media

Tables

View Options

Access options

If you have access to journal content via a personal subscription, university, library, employer or society, select from the options below:


Alternatively, view purchase options below:

Purchase 24 hour online access to view and download content.

Access journal content via a DeepDyve subscription or find out more about this option.

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Full Text

View Full Text