Introduction
An important task in adolescence is to develop a stable concept of self. Self-concept is defined as an estimation or evaluation of one’s own qualities or characteristics (
Bailey, 2003). It has been proposed that the construction of one’s self-concept depends on cognitive abilities and social experiences: interactions with and feedback from others is important for developing a positive and accurate sense of self (
Harter, 2012). Self-concept can therefore be evaluated from a direct personal perspective or from a reflected perspective, where the latter indicates the perceived opinions of others about the self (
Harter, 2012;
Jankowski et al., 2014;
van der Cruijsen et al., 2018,
2019). Autistic individuals often experience difficulties in social situations and communication (
American Psychiatric Association, 2013). They tend to have fewer and qualitatively different affective relationships and can experience difficulties in forming and maintaining close friendships (
Fuentes et al., 2012;
Kuo et al., 2013;
Petrina et al., 2014). Nevertheless, there is still limited comprehension regarding self-concept in adolescents diagnosed with autism spectrum conditions (ASCs).
Several processes regarding self-concept are of interest during adolescence. First, adolescence may be a transition period for self-concept positivity and self-esteem, which are two closely related but different constructs (
Crone et al., 2022). Specifically, self-esteem encompasses the overall evaluation of one’s worth or value as a person (
Bailey, 2003;
Harter, 2012) and is therefore an affective monitor of self-concept (
Crone et al., 2022). In mid-adolescence, there is a relative dip in self-concept positivity (
van der Cruijsen et al., 2018,
2023), and self-esteem is also reported to decline at this age (
Robins & Trzesniewski, 2005), although this may depend on personal situations (
Oshri et al., 2017). Even though some previous studies have shown that autistic adolescents (8–16 years) have lower self-esteem compared to age-matched non-autistic adolescents (
Burrows et al., 2017;
McCauley et al., 2019;
Nguyen et al., 2020;
Pfeifer et al., 2013;
van der Cruijsen & Boyer, 2021), there have not been many studies investigating self-concept positivity among autistic adolescents. A prior study reported that 6- to 12-year-old children with autism, compared to their peers, used fewer positive statements to describe themselves (
Begeer et al., 2008), but no difference was observed in positive self-descriptions in young adults aged 21–28 years with and without autism (Cygan et al., 2018). In this study, we intended to include both indicators of positivity about the self in order to facilitate a better understanding of these constructs in adolescents with autism.
Third, it has been proposed that (perceived) opinions of others about the self are used to construct one’s self-concept (
Harter, 2012;
Van der Cruijsen et al., 2019). Individuals with ASC can have trouble with inferring others’ mental states (i.e. Theory of Mind (ToM);
Begeer & Scheeren, 2021), and it has been proposed that these individuals may have a lower tendency to reason about others’ opinions of the self and to make reflected self-evaluations (
Pfeifer et al., 2009;
Pfeifer & Peake, 2012). Therefore, it would be informative to examine reflected versus direct self-concept in adolescents with autism.
Recent studies used neuroimaging methods to examine self-concept, given that self-concept is strongly dependent on self-report and neural correlates of self-concept evaluations may provide important additional insights. Task-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) allows for the study of neural activity during the process of thinking about one’s own traits. Neuroimaging studies highlighted the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) as a key region involved in self-referential processing in typically developing children, adolescents, and adults (
Denny et al., 2012;
Pfeifer & Peake, 2012;
van Buuren et al., 2020;
Van der Cruijsen et al., 2019). There is conflicting evidence suggesting that autistic individuals may process self-relevant information differently from non-autistic individuals. That is, some studies reported lowered mPFC activation in individuals with autism or with lower autism symptom severity when evaluating self-traits (
Kennedy & Courchesne, 2008;
Kim et al., 2016), whereas other studies showed similar mPFC activation in adolescents with and without autism during self-evaluations (
Burrows et al., 2016; Cygan et al., 2018).
A second brain region that has been linked to integrating perspectives of others in self-concept is the temporal–parietal junction (TPJ) (
Schurz et al., 2014). Neuroimaging studies on self-concept in late adolescents showed that TPJ activation is stronger for reflected than direct self-evaluations (
Pfeifer et al., 2009;
Veroude et al., 2014). Autistic individuals, in contrast, typically show less involvement of TPJ activation in basic mentalizing and ToM tasks compared to individuals without autism (
Kana et al., 2015;
Murdaugh et al., 2014). Therefore, one might expect that involvement of TPJ in reflected self-evaluations is declined in autistic compared to non-autistic adolescents.
Alexithymia
Alexithymia is a sub-clinical condition characterized by difficulties in recognizing and describing one’s own emotions (
Sifneos, 1973). Whereas prevalence in the general population is around 10%–15%, alexithymia co-occurs in 50%–55% of people with ASC (
Hill et al., 2004;
Kinnaird et al., 2019;
Milosavljevic et al., 2016). The alexithymia hypothesis (
Cook et al., 2013) suggests that emotion-related difficulties in individuals with autism originate from alexithymia rather than representing a core feature of autism. This raises the question whether alexithymia rather than autism traits may play a role in self-concept positivity and self-esteem of autistic adolescents.
In addition, alexithymia traits have been related to decreased mentalizing and perspective-taking abilities (
Moriguchi et al., 2006), which was also reflected in neuroimaging studies showing that alexithymia is related to reduced brain activation during empathizing (insula), mentalizing (mPFC), and emotion processing (precuneus) (
Bird et al., 2010;
Moriguchi et al., 2006;
van der Velde et al., 2013). In addition, a structural MRI study in autistic adults has related alexithymia to reduced covariance in social-emotional (frontal-insular) but not social-cognitive (dorsal mPFC, TPJ) networks (
Bernhardt et al., 2014).
The potential role of alexithymia in behavioral and neural measures of reflected self-evaluation has not yet been examined.
Current study
The goal of this study was to provide a comprehensive investigation of self-concept in autistic adolescents by examining (1) self-concept across domains and (2) direct versus reflected self-concept, and using behavioral and neural measurements. Participants evaluated their traits in the academic, physical appearance, and prosocial domain from their own perspective and the reflected perspective of their peers, while undergoing MRI scans. Participants were adolescent males with a clinically established autism diagnosis and typically developing adolescent males aged 12–16 years. Since autistic traits are represented on a spectrum between individuals, we tested for group differences as well as for relationships with autism traits across all participants.
We expected (1a) that self-concept positivity, especially in the prosocial domain, and self-esteem would be lower in autistic compared to non-autistic adolescents, and would be negatively related to the number of autism traits (
Bauminger et al., 2004;
McCauley et al., 2019;
van der Cruijsen & Boyer, 2021;
Williamson et al., 2008). Regarding reflected versus direct self-concept, we expected (1b) higher similarity in non-autistic compared to autistic adolescents, or in participants with fewer autism traits (
Pfeifer et al., 2009;
Pfeifer & Peake, 2012). As alexithymia may explain emotion-related problems in autistic individuals (
Cook et al., 2013), and it has been found to be related to decreased perspective-taking skills (
Moriguchi et al., 2006), we exploratively tested (1c) whether alexithymia would explain lowered self-concept, lower self-esteem, or larger differences between direct and reflected self-concepts above autism traits.
On the neural level, we expected (2a) self-related mPFC activation in adolescents with and without autism (
Burrows et al., 2016; Cygan et al., 2018). Previous studies were conflicted regarding potential differences in this activation between adolescents with and without autism. Therefore, here we tested exploratively for group differences and relationships with autism traits across both groups. Next, we expected (2b) that TPJ activation for reflected self-evaluations, and differentiation in TPJ activation for reflected versus direct self-evaluations would be stronger in non-autistic adolescents compared to adolescents with autism, or in participants with fewer autism traits (
Kana et al., 2015;
Kennedy & Courchesne, 2008;
Lombardo et al., 2011;
Murdaugh et al., 2014). Potentially, given the difficulties with social skills adolescents with autism often face, differences in neural activation between autistic and non-autistic adolescents mainly become apparent in the prosocial domain. Last, as alexithymia traits have been related to reduced neural activation for affective and mentalizing processes (
Bird et al., 2010;
Moriguchi et al., 2006;
van der Velde et al., 2013), we tested (2c) whether alexithymia explained lowered mPFC and TPJ activation above autism traits.
Methods
Participants
Participants were 40 adolescent autistic males and 37 non-autistic peers aged between 12.1 and 16.9 years. In total, five participants with and three participants without autism were excluded due to not completing the MRI scans (
NAutism = 1), or excessive head movements during the MRI scans (
NAutism = 4,
NNon-Autism = 3), resulting in a final sample of 35 adolescent males with autism and 34 non-autistic peers (see
Table 1). IQ was estimated using the two subtests “vocabulary” and “block design” of the Dutch Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III-NL;
Kort et al., 2005), which are known to correlate strongly to full-scale IQ (M = 100, SD = 15;
Sattler, 2001). Estimated IQ scores ranged from 80 to 135 and did not differ between groups (
t(67) = 1.18,
p = 0.241). See
Table 1 for information on ethnicity and gross annual income.
Participants with ASC were recruited by sending an email to parents of boys aged 12 to 16 years, who were registered at the Netherlands Autism Register (NAR;
https://nar.vu.nl/english/what-is-the-nar). A clinical
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., text rev.; DSM-IV-TR;
White, 2012) or
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed., DSM-5) diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (ASD;
American Psychiatric Association, 2013) was previously, independent of this study, determined in all participants by a psychiatrist or certified psychologist. Twenty autistic adolescents had co-occurring diagnoses, of whom six adolescents had two. Comorbid diagnoses were dyslexia (9), ADHD (8), post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (2), sensory integration disorder (2), Gilles de la Tourette (1), anxiety disorder (1), behavioral disorder not otherwise specified (1), and nonverbal learning disability (1). Fifteen adolescents used medication. Twelve participants used methylphenidate, of which one additionally used citalopram and four additionally used antipsychotics (aripiprazole, risperidone (2x), pipamperone). In addition, one participant only used dexamphetamine, one only used atomoxetine, and one only used aripiprazole. Participants who used medication were asked to take medication as usual before the lab visit to minimize possible influences of co-occurring problems such as attention problems on task performance.
Non-autistic participants were 12- to 16-year-old males (selected on matching age and gender) who participated in the first timepoint of a larger study (
van der Cruijsen et al., 2018). None of the participants had any clinical diagnosis, or used medication, as was reported by parents over the phone during study inclusion, and self-reported by the adolescent in the questionnaire.
Self-report measures
Autism traits
To measure the number of autism traits, participants completed the abridged version of the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ-short;
Hoekstra et al., 2011). The questionnaire consists of 28 items which can be answered on a scale of 1 (definitely agree) to 4 (definitely disagree). Items were scored as either 0 or 1, with answer options 1 and 2 coded as “0” and answer options 3 and 4 coded as “1,” or vice versa for items that needed to be reverse-coded. The internal consistency was α = 0.74 for participants with autism, and α = 0.81 for non-autistic participants.
Alexithymia
To measure alexithymia, participants completed the Alexithymia Questionnaire for Children (AQC;
Rieffe et al., 2006), consisting of 20 items on which participants could respond on a scale of 1 to 3 (1 =not true, 2 = a bit true, 3 = true). This questionnaire consists of three subscales: difficulty identifying feelings (DIF; e.g. “I am often confused about what emotion I am feeling”), difficulty describing feelings (DDF; e.g. “It is difficult for me to find the right words for my feelings”), and externally oriented thinking (EOT; e.g. “I prefer to just let things happen rather than to understand why they turned out that way”). The internal consistency of the subscales was DIF: α = 0.74, DDF: α = 0.72, EOT: α = 0.34 for participants with autism, and DIF: α = 0.61, DDF: α = 0.80, EOT: α = 0.62 for participants without autism. Given the low reliability of the EOT subscale, we excluded the EOT subscale from further analyses.
Self-esteem
To measure adolescents’ self-esteem, participants completed the Dutch version of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES), consisting of 10 items on which participants could respond on a scale of 1 (completely not true) to 4 (completely true) (
Veldhuis et al., 2014). Internal consistency was α = 0.89 for participants with autism and α = 0.73 for participants without autism.
Task design
FMRI self-concept task
Participants evaluated the extent to which sentences describing positive and negative traits in academic, physical, and prosocial domains fit them on a scale of 1 (not at all) to 4 (completely) (see
Figure 1). The task consisted of two experimental conditions and one control condition. In the experimental conditions, participants evaluated their traits from their own (direct self-evaluation condition) or their peers’ perspective (reflected self-evaluation condition). Trait sentences were the same in both conditions (i.e. “I am smart,” “I am unattractive”), but in the reflected condition were preceded by the words: “My peers think about me that …” Participants evaluated 60 trait sentences in both conditions: 20 sentences per domain, of which half were positive and half were negative. In the control condition, participants categorized 10 positive and 10 negative trait sentences different to those in the experimental conditions into one of four categories: (1) school, (2) social, (3) appearance, and (4) I don’t know. For behavioral analyses of self-concept positivity, responses to negative items were reverse-scored and averaged together with responses on positive items.
The three conditions were completed in separate runs of which the order was counterbalanced between participants. Within the runs, trials were presented in a pseudorandomized order regarding domains, optimized using Optseq (
Dale, 1999). Optseq was also used to add jittered intertrial intervals, which varied between 0 and 4.4 s. Each trial began with a fixation cross shown for 400 ms, after which the stimulus was presented for 4600 ms. When participants successfully responded to the sentence within this timeframe, the number they chose turned yellow for the remaining stimulus time in order to assure participants that their choice had been registered. If participants failed to respond, they were shown the phrase “Too late!” for 1000 ms. These trials were modeled separately and were not included in the analyses. Too late responses for adolescents with autism and typically developing adolescents, respectively, occurred on 1.5% and 1.4% of trials in the direct condition, on 2.7% and 2.4% of trials in the reflected condition, and on 1.1% and 0.9% of trials in the control condition. Differences in missed responses were not significant between groups (all
p > 0.687).
FMRI preprocessing
Data were analyzed using SPM8 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London) for comparison with previously published studies (
Van der Cruijsen et al., 2019). Functional scans were corrected for slice-timing acquisition and rigid body movement differences. Structural and functional volumes were spatially normalized to T1 templates by an algorithm using a 12-parameter affine transformation together with a nonlinear transformation involving cosine basis functions, resampling the volumes to 3-mm cubic voxels. Templates were based on the MNI305 stereotaxic space (
Cocosco et al., 1996). Functional volumes were spatially smoothed with a 6-mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) isotropic Gaussian kernel.
Task effects for each participant were estimated using the general linear model (GLM) in SPM8. The fMRI data were modeled as a series of zero duration events convolved with the hemodynamic response function (HRF). Modeled events of interest for the direct condition were “Direct-Academic-Positive,” “Direct-Academic-Negative,” “Direct-Physical-Positive,” “Direct-Physical-Negative,” “Direct-Prosocial-Positive,” and “Direct-Prosocial-Negative.” The same events were modeled for the reflected condition. For the control condition, only one event of interest was modeled: “Control” (collapsed across domains and valences). Trials for which participants failed to respond in time were modeled as events of no interest. The events were used as covariates in a GLM, together with a basic set of cosine functions that high-pass filtered the data. Six motion regressors were added to the model. Participants who moved more than 3 mm in any direction were excluded from the analyses (n = 4 autistic adolescents and n = 3 non-autistic adolescents). The resulting contrast images, computed on a subject-by-subject basis, were submitted to group analyses.
For motion differences between groups, see
Table 1. Both across groups and within both groups separately, motion was not related to autism traits, alexithymia traits, or self-esteem (all
p > 0.063). Controlling for motion in all analyses did not change the results.
FMRI whole-brain analyses
See supplement for fMRI data acquisition. To investigate our hypotheses, we first performed whole-brain one-sample
t tests for the contrast Self (Direct + Reflected) > Control, separately for both groups. Subsequently, we performed a whole-brain two-sample
t test for the difference in this same contrast between the groups. Family-wise error (FWE) cluster correction was applied in these analyses. To further investigate our hypotheses regarding mPFC and TPJ activation, we extracted parameter estimates from 3 regions of interest (ROIs; 8-mm spheres) using the MarsBaR ROI toolbox: mPFC (
x = −6,
y = 50,
z = 4), right TPJ (x = −53,
y = −59,
z = 20), and left TPJ (
x = 56,
y = −56,
z = 18). These ROIs were based on previous meta-analyses (
Denny et al., 2012;
Schurz et al., 2014) and have been used in our early study on self-concept development in adolescence (
Van der Cruijsen et al., 2019).
Behavioral and ROI analyses
Repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were conducted to examine group differences in behavior and neural activation in the three ROIs. Next, hierarchical regression analyses were conducted for two purposes. First, with these analyses we examined whether behavior and neural activation related to autism traits regardless of diagnosis. Second, by adding alexithymia traits in the next step of the regression, we tested whether alexithymia would explain additional variance in self-concept and self-related neural activation above autism traits.
Results were corrected for multiple comparisons using a Bonferroni method adjusting for correlated variables (
http://www.quantitativeskills.com/sisa/calculations/bonfer.htm;
Perneger, 1998;
Sankoh et al., 1997). For the hierarchical regressions including behavioral measures, the correlation between the seven outcome variables was
r = 0.314, which resulted in an adjusted significance level (two-sided) of α = 0.013. For the hierarchical regressions including the 12 ROI measures in the contrasts Self > Control, the average correlation was
r = 0.503, resulting in an adjusted significance level of α = 0.0145. Last, for the hierarchical regressions including the 12 ROI measures in the contrasts Reflected > Direct, the average correlation was
r = 0.4659, resulting in an adjusted significance level of α = 0.013. Even though all hierarchical regression analyses answered one overarching question (whether alexithymia rather than autism traits were related to indicators of self-concept and self-esteem), there were three variables of interest in these analyses. Therefore, we have reported when results would not survive an additional Bonferroni correction (α = 0.013/3 = α = 0.0043; α = 0.0145/3 = α = 0.0048; and α = 0.013/3 = α = 0.0043, respectively).
Community involvement statement
Community members were not actively involved in the construction of this study. However, every year, the NAR exchanges ideas on relevant research topics with stakeholders such as autistic adults and parents of children with autism. The NAR also has several autistic team members.