Skip to main content

[]

Intended for healthcare professionals
Skip to main content
Free access
Research article
First published online November 21, 2014

Efficacy and Feasibility of Functional Upper Extremity Task-Specific Training for Older Adults With and Without Cognitive Impairment

Abstract

Background. Although functional task-specific training is a viable approach for upper extremity neurorehabilitation, its appropriateness for older populations is unclear. If task-specific training is to be prescribed to older adults, it must be efficacious and feasible, even in patients with cognitive decline due to advancing age. Objective. This cross-sectional study tested the efficacy and feasibility of upper extremity task-specific training in older adults, including those with lower cognitive scores. Methods. Fifty older adults (age 65-89 years) without any confounding neuromuscular impairment were randomly assigned to a training group or no-training group. The training group completed 3 days (dosage = 2250 repetitions) of a functional upper extremity motor task (simulated feeding) with their nondominant hand; the no-training group completed no form of training at all. Both groups’ task performance (measured as trial time) was tested at pre- and posttest, and the training group was retested 1 month later. Efficacy was determined by rate, amount, and retention of training-related improvement, and compared across levels of cognitive status. Feasibility was determined by participants’ tolerance of the prescribed training dose. Results. The training group was able to complete the training dose without adverse responses and showed a significant rate, amount, and retention of improvement compared with the no-training group. Cognitive status did not alter results, although participants with lower scores on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment were slower overall. Conclusions. Task-specific training may be appropriate for improving upper extremity function in older adults, yet future work in older patients with specific neurological conditions is needed.

Introduction

Functional task-specific training is emerging as a viable approach to upper extremity neurorehabilitation, particularly following stroke.1-4 During training, individuals repetitively practice tasks that are meaningful to daily life, such as manipulating a lock and key, opening and closing a door, or using an utensil.5-7 The clinical utility of this approach, however, depends on its ability to promote and capture changes in motor function across a wide age range of patients, including older adults, given the high percentage of adults over age 65 currently receiving therapy.8
There are 2 important factors that must be considered when addressing issues related to neurorehabilitation in older adults. First, the “normal” aging process9 may impede the course of standard neurorehabilitation. For example, older adults tend to learn new motor skills at a slower rate and to a lesser extent than young adults.10,11 Thus, older patients may require more training to induce a therapeutic effect (ie, efficacy). Furthermore, if more task-specific training is needed for older patients, then they must be able to tolerate these extended training sessions (ie, feasibility). Little research has, however, addressed the efficacy and feasibility of task-specific training in older adults.
Second, “abnormal” aging (eg, mild cognitive impairment, dementia) can be quite common in late adulthood,12 which may further reduce an older patient’s ability to perform and/or learn a motor skill.13 For example, gait disturbances become more apparent in older patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease when under dual-task conditions, but not under single-task conditions.14 This interaction between cognition and movement has not, however, been systematically studied in the neurorehabilitation literature.
Therefore, the primary aim of this study was to examine the efficacy and feasibility of upper extremity task-specific training in a sample of older adults without confounding neuromuscular impairment. We hypothesized that adults age 65 years and older would show measurable learning effects (efficacy) in response to a high dose of training on a functional upper extremity motor task without adverse effects (feasibility). To address the question of “abnormal” aging and neurorehabilitation, a secondary aim of the study was to investigate the impact of cognitive impairment on training in these older adults. We hypothesized that lower cognitive status would reduce the efficacy and feasibility of the training dose, compared with normal cognitive status.

Methods

Participants

Fifty-five older adults (≥65 years old) were recruited from the local community and assisted-living facilities. Recruitment was based on individuals who contacted the lab with interest in participating as a result of approved postings throughout Cache County. Based on preliminary trial time data (8 participants), a priori power calculations indicated that at least 32 participants (16 training and 16 no-training) were needed to provide 80% power with an α level of .05. Exclusion criteria included one or more self-reported neurological conditions (eg, Parkinson’s disease, Huntington’s disease, stroke, or transient ischemic attack) or acute or chronic musculoskeletal conditions that could affect motor function. Based on these criteria, 5 participants were excluded from this study. Informed consent was obtained prior to study participation, and this study was approved by the university institutional review board.

Measures

Participants’ cognitive and sensorimotor functions were characterized prior to the task-specific training. Global cognitive status was measured with the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA15), which is a reliable, easily administered, and brief cognitive screening test (max score = 30; “normal” score cutoff ≥ 26). Tactile sensation was measured with Semmes Weinstein monofilaments (Touch-Test, North Coast Medical, Inc, Gilroy, CA) at the distal end of the left and right index fingers only. Maximal grip strength of the nondominant hand was tested via hand dynamometer (Jamar, Sammons-Preston-Rolyan, Bolingbrook, IL16,17) as the average of 3 consecutive measurements. Hand dominance was determined using a modified Edinburgh Handedness Questionnaire.18
The functional upper extremity motor task used in this study was a task that simulated feeding. As noted in our previous studies,19,20 this multijoint coordination task has been adapted from the simulated feeding subtest of a clinical assessment21 that objectively assesses hand function for activities of daily living. This task required participants to spoon beans (kidney, raw) at a time from a center proximal “start” cup to 3 distal “target” cups as fast as possible. The cups (9.5 cm in diameter) were secured to a board (60.5 cm × 40.0 cm), with 3 target cups secured radially at 45°, 90°, and 135° around the start cup at a distance of 16 cm (Figure 1).22,23 The start cup was oriented along the participant’s midline and 15 cm in front of the seated participant.
Figure 1. Top view of the simulated feeding task.
Start and target cups were secured to a board that was centered at participants’ midline 15 cm in front of them. Spoon location was 5 cm lateral to the start cup. Target cups were distal to the start cup at a radius of 16 cm and at angles of 45°, 90°, and 135° relative the start cup. The spoon start/end location was 5 cm lateral to the start cup. Note: Setup shown is for testing the left hand.
One repetition of the motor task consisted of spooning 2 beans at once from the start cup to a target cup with the nondominant hand,19,24 with one trial equaling 15 repetitions. During each trial, participants moved first to the ipsilateral target cup, next to the center target cup, and then to the contralateral cup, relative to the hand used (left vs right). They repeated this sequence 5 times to complete the trial. Each trial began when the participants picked up the spoon (plastic, 5.21 g) and ended when they returned the spoon to its start location (5 cm lateral to the start cup). The measure of performance for each trial was the time taken to complete the 15 repetitions (ie, “trial time”), with faster times indicating better performance. All trials were timed to the nearest 100th of a second via stopwatch. In contrast to some experimental or therapeutic approaches,25 participants were given no explicit feedback (ie, knowledge of performance or results) after each trial, and they were not encouraged to adopt any specified pattern of upper extremity kinematics during training.

Experimental Protocol

Task-specific training on the functional motor task was completed over 3 consecutive days (Figure 2). All data were collected in an individual setting (rather than group) within the Motor Rehabilitation and Learning Laboratory at Utah State University. Each assessment described above, including the MoCA, was completed on Day 1, prior to any motor task performance. Following the pretest measurement of “baseline” nondominant hand motor performance, participants were randomly assigned to a training group or no-training group via simple randomization26 to avoid bias in this open study. Overall, the dose of task-specific training for the training group was 2250 total repetitions (15 repetitions/trial × 50 trials/day × 3 days). This dose has been shown previously to be feasible and efficacious for adults with chronic mild to moderate hemiparesis after stroke.6,20,27,28
Figure 2. Experimental schedule for training and no-training groups.

Dependent Variables

Feasibility was determined by the participants’ completion (and therefore “tolerance”) of the prescribed dose of task-specific training without adverse effects, such as fatigue and boredom. Efficacy was evaluated by the rate, amount, and retention of improvement in task performance. To measure the rate of improvement in response to task-specific training, we generated performance curves by plotting trial time (measured in seconds) as a function of trial number (trial 1-150), and modeled the data using an exponential decay function:
y=a+bex/c,
where a is the final trial time value that the exponential decay function approaches (ie, asymptote), b is the scale of the learning from the first trial time to the value a, x is the trial number, and c is the rate at which learning occurs (ie, the decay constant). In this model, the exponential decay constant, 1/c, is the number of trials needed to obtain (1 − e−1) or 63.2% of the final learned amount (or asymptote, a). This approach has been used previously to quantify upper extremity motor adaptation in healthy and clinical populations.29-31 Thus, we used the value c as the rate of improvement in this study. Given the design of this study, no data on rate of trial-by-trial improvement were collected in the no-training group, as they did not complete any training.
Immediately following training on Day 3, participants in the training group completed an additional trial of the functional motor task to establish their posttest performance (Figure 2). Posttest evaluation occurred within 5 minutes of the last training trial (eg, a short break was given based on participant request). Participants in the no-training group were also tested on Day 3, although they did not train in between their pre- and posttest sessions. The difference in pre- and posttest trial times for the training and no-training groups was used to measure the amount of improvement in response to training (see section Statistical Analyses), and to assess whether test–retest alone induced any motor learning. Grip strength was also measured again in both the training and no-training groups at posttest to ensure that any improvements in motor performance were due to task-specific training rather than changes in strength.
To measure the retention of improvement, participants in the training group were then retested 1 month (30-35 days) after Day 3 on 2 trials (15 repetitions each) of the functional motor task. As with improvement rate, no retention data were collected in the no-training group, given that preliminary data and previous studies19 showed no short-term change in performance when no training has occurred. Each training group participant’s retention at 1 month was expressed as the average of his/her 2 trial times to account for performance stability and washout (as recommended in earlier studies32,33). The difference in pretest and retention trial times for the training groups was used to quantify retention, as was any difference between posttest and retention.
Efficacy was further tested by comparing the rate, amount, and retention of improvement based on participant’s global cognitive status. Participants were assigned post hoc to 1 of 2 MoCA categories, based on the clinical cutoff score of 26.15 Although MoCA scores can be analyzed as a continuous measure in cases of large sample sizes and broad score ranges (0-30), current convention treats MoCA performance as a dichotomous variable,34-36 particularly in cross-sectional studies. Thus, participants were categorized as <26 or ≥26, and we operationally defined “cognitive impairment” as a score <26 for purposes of this study. To account for any potential relationship between cognitive impairment and age, we calculated a Pearson product–moment correlation coefficient (r2) across participants using least squares regression and determined whether there was a significant linear relationship within our sample between MoCA score and age. Since the “oldest old” (85+ years37) typically have (a) lower cognitive scores and (b) slower movements (ie, more bradykinetic) than the “young old” (65-74 years38), we used this analysis to ensure that any differences in trial times associated with MoCA scores were not simply the result of the oldest participants moving the slowest.

Statistical Analyses

The SAS statistical software program JMP 10.0 (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC) was used for all statistical analysis (α = .05). T tests and χ2 tests were used to determine whether the training and no-training groups were matched for age, sex, general cognitive status, tactile sensation, and pretest nondominant hand grip strength.
To examine the efficacy of training, we first calculated the rate of improvement for each participant in the training group and used a 95% confidence interval to test whether the average rate was significantly different from zero. This determined whether participants in the training group had a significant rate of improvement. To test whether cognitive impairment affected the rate of improvement due to training, we compared rates between MoCA categories (<26 vs ≥26) using an independent T test.
A second test of efficacy compared of the amount of improvement in trial time between sessions (pretest vs posttest) based on group (training vs no-training) and MoCA category (<26 vs ≥26) using a 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). Session was a within-subject factor, while group and MoCA category were between-subject factors. This analysis further tested whether cognitive impairment affected the amount of improvement due to training.
Our third test of efficacy compared the retention of improvement at 1 month to performance at earlier sessions (pretest and posttest) based on MoCA category using a 3 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA. This final analysis also tested whether cognitive impairment affected the retention of improvement due to training. In all the aforementioned analyses, post hoc analyses were conducted when warranted using the Tukey–Kramer Honestly Significant Difference (HSD) test.

Results

Group Characteristics

Table 1 summarizes the characteristics for the training (n = 23) and no-training (n = 27) groups. The groups were not significantly different on global cognition (P = .52), age (P = .58), sex (P = .20), sensation level (P = .47), nondominant grip strength at pretest (P = .83) or at posttest (P = .83, not shown). Table 1 also shows that we were able to recruit evenly across a wide range of ages (65-89 years). Regression analysis indicated a weak relationship between MoCA score and age (r2 = .25).
Table 1. Group Characteristics.
GroupMoCAa, Median (Range)Age (Years), Median (Range)SexNondominant (Tested) HandbNondominant Hand SensationcNondominant Hand Grip Strength (kg)d, Mean (SD)
Training (n = 23)25 (18-29)75 (65-89)7 males, 16 females12 left, 2 right2.83 (n = 9); 3.61 (n = 12); 4.31 (n = 2)24.9 (9.9)
No-training (n =27)26 (7-30)76 (65-88)13 males, 14 females26 left, 1 right2.83 (n = 14); 3.61 (n = 11); 4.31 (n = 1); 6.61 (n = 1)25.5 (10.0)
Abbreviation: MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment.
a
Maximum MoCA score = 30. Scores above 26 are considered normal.
b
Hand tested was the nondominant hand, determined by Edinburgh Handedness Questionnaire.
c
Sensation of the index fingertip, palmar surface, expressed as lowest (finest) detectable Semmes-Weinstein monofilament thickness; 2.83, 3.61, 4.31, and 6.61 are manufacturer-assigned numbers, with higher values indicating stiffer monofilaments, according to formula: nominal value = log10[bending force (in milligrams) × l0]. n = number of participants.
d
Nondominant hand grip strength, measured at pretest via dynamometer. Average of 3 consecutive measurements.

Efficacy of Training

Figure 3 illustrates the rate, amount, and retention of improvement due to training. The rate of improvement can be seen in the mean performance curve for the training group (Figure 3A). In addition, the 95% confidence interval ranged from 0.030 to 0.088, indicating that the learning rates were significantly different from zero. The mean rate was such that maximal learning effects were achieved after ~27 trials (or 405 repetitions), although some participants learned much faster (eg, 101 repetitions) and some much slower (eg, 2483 repetitions). Figure 3B shows the amount of improvement in trial time for both the training and no-training groups from pretest to posttest. Repeated-measures ANOVA indicated a significant interaction between session (pretest vs posttest) and group (training vs no-training) for trial time (F1,46 = 13.94, P < .001). Post hoc analyses revealed that mean trial time for the training group was significantly lower (ie, faster) at posttest compared with pretest (P < .001), whereas the no-training group showed no significant change from pretest to posttest (P = .87). Moreover, mean trial times at pretest were similar between the training and no-training groups (P = .99), indicating that both groups’ baseline performances were comparable at the start of the study. The lack of change in grip strength from pretest to posttest (see section Group Characteristics) also suggests that the training group’s improvements were not attributable to increased strength due to training.
Figure 3. (A) Mean trial times for the training group over the course of 150 training trials (50 trials/day × 3 days). (B) Mean trial time at pretest and posttest for the no-training and training groups. Posttest trial times were significantly lower in the training group only (**P < .01; n.s. = non-significant). (C) Mean trial time at retention (1-month follow-up) for the training group only. Retention trial time was significantly lower than pretest (***P < .0001), but not significantly different from posttest.
(A-C) Faster trial times indicate better performance. Error bars indicate standard error.
In addition to significant rates and amounts of improvement, Figure 3C shows mean trial time for participants in the training group 1 month later (“retention”). Not only did trial times improve from pretest to posttest (Figure 3B), but the improvements were also retained 1 month later, as shown by the main effect of session (F2,21 = 23.17, P < .0001) on trial time. Trial times at 1 month after training were still significantly lower (ie, faster) compared to pretest (P < .0001), and were not significantly different from posttest (P = .41).

Feasibility of Training

All eligible participants completed the study (n = 50). However, 2 participants in the training group (8.7%) were unable to complete the entire dose over 3 days due to self-reported fatigue or lack of interest on Day 1 (ie, asked to stop and complete remaining trials on a consecutive, fourth training day). Four participants in the training group (17.4%) also verbally expressed, impromptu at the end of Day 3, that they had experienced boredom at some point. No other adverse effects were reported in this study in either the training or no-training groups. Thus, the training dose evaluated in this study was tolerated by a range of older adults.

Relationship Between Cognitive Impairment and Efficacy/Feasibility

While Figure 3 shows the overall comparison between the training and no-training groups, it does not differentiate between levels of cognitive status. Rather, Figure 4 shows comparisons within groups between participants with and without cognitive impairment. Although we hypothesized reduced efficacy/feasibility in those with lower cognitive status, our data collectively showed no effect of MoCA category on any measure. As illustrated by the performance curves in Figure 4A, there was no significant difference in the improvement rates between participants with MoCA scores <26 (mean ± SE =.051 ± .02) and ≥26 (.070 ± .02), P = .62. Thus, regardless of MoCA category, participants in the training group learned the functional motor task at comparable rates.
Figure 4. (A) Mean trial times over the course of 150 training trials within each Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) category. Error bars have been omitted for visual clarity. (B) Mean trial time at pretest and posttest for the no-training and training groups. Posttest trial times were significantly lower in the training group only in both MoCA categories (*P < .05; ***P < .0001; n.s. = nonsignificant). (C) Mean trial time at retention (1-month follow-up) for the training group only. Retention trial time was significantly lower than pretest in both MoCA categories (*P < .05; **P < .01), but not significantly different from posttest.
(A-C) Faster trial times indicate better performance. Error bars indicate standard error. Dark gray indicates data from participants with MoCA scores below 26; light gray indicates those with MoCA scores at or above 26.
MoCA category also did not affect the amount of improvement due to training (Figure 4B). Repeated-measures ANOVA indicated (a) no significant 3-way interaction between session (pretest vs posttest), group (training vs no-training), and MoCA (<26 vs ≥26) on trial time (F1,46 = 0.002, P = .96) and (b) no significant 2-way interactions between group and MoCA category (F1,46 = 0.46, P = .50) or session and MoCA category (F1,46 = 0.85, P = .36). There was, however, a significant main effect of MoCA (F1,46 = 4.60, P < .05) on trial time. Thus, regardless of group or session, trial times were higher (ie, slower) for participants with MoCA scores below the cutoff. This bradykinetic effect is also captured in Figure 4A, such that the performance curve for the <26 category is shifted upward relative to that for the ≥26 category.
Figure 4C further shows the lack of effect of MoCA category on training efficacy. When comparing retention of improvement, there was no significant interaction between session and MoCA category (F2,21 = 0.59, P = .55) or a main effect of MoCA category (F1,21 = 2.56, P = .13) on trial time. Thus, regardless of MoCA category, participants retained their training-related improvements in motor performance one month after training.
Although 91% of the participants in this study tolerated the dose of training over the 3-day administration period (see section Group Characteristics), the 2 participants in the training group who required an additional fourth day due to fatigue on Day 1 had MoCA scores below 26. They nevertheless completed the full training dose, and all other participants with scores <26 tolerated the 3-day training period, suggesting that the training’s feasibility is not affected by cognitive impairment.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to test the efficacy and feasibility of functional upper extremity task-specific training for adults age 65 years or older without any confounding neuromuscular impairment. Results showed that a high dose of training on a functional motor task (2250 repetitions of simulated feeding) was efficacious, yielding significant learning effects that were measured as the rate, amount, and retention of improvement, and was feasible, based on their completion and tolerance, without adverse effects. Moreover, participants’ cognitive status and strength did not affect these measures or their ability to tolerate the dose. Thus, task-specific training may be appropriate for upper extremity neurorehabilitation in older populations, including the oldest old and those with some degree of cognitive impairment.
An important finding from this study is that the improvements in trial time associated with a set dose of task-specific training were retained 1 month later. In prior studies, older adults have been shown to partially retain experience-dependent adaptations for 1 to 7 days after upper extremity training.39-41 Participants in those studies were required to specifically adapt their motor output in order to reduce reaching errors induced by systematic, computer-driven perturbations in visual feedback (ie, visuomotor adaptation). Results from the current study not only extend the potential timeframe of retention for older adults (up to 1 month) but also broadens the types of tasks that can be used to probe for retention. Our functional motor task allowed participants to manipulate a physical object (the spoon) using whichever grip type they wanted, and to transport the beans using whichever upper extremity movement pattern they wanted in order to successfully complete a trial (ie, a “discovery learning” approach42). This approach reflects how a patient may relearn a functional motor skill in real-world settings, either in therapy or at home, and may be useful for future investigations of motor learning that are clinically relevant.
The learning effects observed in this study, namely, the rate, amount, and retention, may be partly due to the relatively high dose of functional upper extremity task-specific training (2250 repetitions over 3 days) that was administered in this study, compared with standard clinical rehabilitative practice.43,44 Recent research has started to quantify the dose–response relationship for upper extremity neurorehabilitation treatments very similar to the approach taken in this study,45,46 and has documented across a number of clinical trials and controlled experiments that patient groups who receive more therapy improve beyond those who receive less. Those studies benchmark an important step in providing an evidence-based approach to physical rehabilitation, whereby clinicians can and should prescribe doses of therapy (defined as the amount of practice/repetitions rather than the time spent in therapy; see Lang et al44 for rationale) based on a set of standard criteria. Our results also provide evidence that older adults, including the oldest-old and those with some level of cognitive impairment, can tolerate high doses of upper extremity task-specific training that have been feasible and effective for other neurologic populations.6,28
Although the older adults in this study appeared to tolerate the tested dose of training, they did not have any known conditions or diseases that are often associated with sensorimotor or neuromuscular deficits (eg, stroke, Parkinson’s disease). While this sample allowed us to test for “pure” age-related effects in the absence of confounding comorbidities, these results are limited and should be interpreted with caution. Other limitations to this study include the short duration of training (3 days) and the lack of current knowledge regarding how improvements on the feeding task are related to other functional or clinical outcomes (eg, Fugl-Meyer, activities of daily living, etc). We also acknowledge that the primary “outcome” measure and the training protocol were identical in this study; this was necessary, however, to quantify efficacy in the context of motor learning.47 Thus, future studies are needed to address (a) the expected outcomes of older patients with specific neurological conditions in response to task-specific training as an intervention; (b) the selection, prescription, and administration of tasks by clinicians within therapy; and (c) the generalization of training to other tasks. Ongoing studies from this laboratory are in fact investigating the degree to which the learning effects shown here are transferrable in older adults, relative to younger participants with and without a history of stroke.19,20,24
While global cognitive status did not appear to affect the extent to which participants in this study learned (or retained) the functional motor task, the lack of interaction between MoCA category and the other factors in this study does not preclude the role of cognitive factors in neurorehabilitation. In fact, our finding that participants with lower MoCA scores had slower movements overall suggests that their bradykinesia may be in part related to cognitive factors,48,49 and not simply an effect of age (ie, the oldest participants having the longest trial times), given the weak relationship between MoCA score and age in our sample (noted in section Group Characteristics). Nevertheless, the MoCA (a) is a gross measure of global cognition, (b) does not thoroughly assess visuospatial abilities or working memory, and (c) does not at all test aspects of cognition that are highly related to motor learning, specifically implicit memory.50,51 Thus, its ability to detect or predict deficits in motor learning is minimal. Our data showed, however, that some participants in this study learned the task faster (ie, in fewer trials) than others. Although experimental brain imaging techniques are able to correlate specific neural features with rates of motor adaptation,52 there is a current lack of clinical neuropsychological tools that capture implicit memory function directly. Thus, one’s rate of motor learning may, in fact, be useful in quantifying one’s implicit memory. Furthermore, it may potentially serve as a clinical predictor of one’s success in therapy.53 More work is needed to establish the extent to which measures of motor learning (such as rate, amount, etc) are not only related to specific cognitive deficits that may be present in neurological populations but can also indicate neurorehabilitative outcomes in these populations.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This work was supported in part by the Utah State University Office of Research and Graduate Studies (RC #28037) and the Marriner S. Eccles Foundation.

References

1. Bayona NA, Bitensky J, Salter K, Teasell R. The role of task-specific training in rehabilitation therapies. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2005;12(3):58-65.
2. Timmermans AA, Spooren AI, Kingma H, Seelen HA. Influence of task-oriented training content on skilled arm-hand performance in stroke: a systematic review. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2010;24:858-870.
3. Veerbeek JM, van Wegen E, van Peppen R, et al. What is the evidence for physical therapy poststroke? A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One. 2014;9:e87987.
4. Zeiler SR, Krakauer JW. The interaction between training and plasticity in the poststroke brain. Curr Opin Neurol. 2013;26:609-616.
5. Arya KN, Verma R, Garg RK, Sharma VP, Agarwal M, Aggarwal GG. Meaningful task-specific training (MTST) for stroke rehabilitation: a randomized controlled trial. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2012;19:193-211.
6. Birkenmeier RL, Prager EM, Lang CE. Translating animal doses of task-specific training to people with chronic stroke in 1-hour therapy sessions: a proof-of-concept study. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2010;24:620-635.
7. Hubbard IJ, Parsons MW, Neilson C, Carey LM. Task-specific training: evidence for and translation to clinical practice. Occup Ther Int. 2009;16:175-189.
8. Knapp D, Russell T, Byrum C, Waters S. Entry-Level Practice Analysis Update for Physical Therapist Licensure Examinations Offered by the Federation of State Boards of Physical Therapy. Alexandria, VA: Federation of State Boards of Physical Therapy; 2007.
9. Morack J, Ram N, Fauth EB, Gerstorf D. Multidomain trajectories of psychological functioning in old age: a longitudinal perspective on (uneven) successful aging. Dev Psychol. 2013;49:2309-2324.
10. Boyd LA, Vidoni ED, Siengsukon CF. Multidimensional motor sequence learning is impaired in older but not younger or middle-aged adults. Phys Ther. 2008;88:351-362.
11. Tunney N, Taylor LF, Gaddy M, et al. Aging and motor learning of a functional motor task. Phys Occup Ther Geriat. 2004;21(3):1-16.
12. Hanninen T, Koivisto K, Reinikainen KJ, et al. Prevalence of ageing-associated cognitive decline in an elderly population. Age Ageing. 1996;25:201-205.
13. Robertson DA, Savva GM, Kenny RA. Frailty and cognitive impairment—a review of the evidence and causal mechanisms. Ageing Res Rev. 2013;12:840-851.
14. Muir SW, Gopaul K, Montero Odasso MM. The role of cognitive impairment in fall risk among older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Age Ageing. 2012;41:299-308.
15. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:695-699.
16. Andrews AW, Thomas MW, Bohannon RW. Normative values for isometric muscle force measurements obtained with hand-held dynamometers. Phys Ther. 1996;76:248-259.
17. Schmidt RT, Toews JV. Grip strength as measured by the Jamar dynamometer. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1970;51:321-327.
18. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97-113.
19. Schaefer SY, Lang CE. Using dual tasks to test immediate transfer of training between naturalistic movements: a proof-of-principle study. J Mot Behav. 2012;44:313-327.
20. Schaefer SY, Patterson CB, Lang CE. Transfer of training between distinct motor tasks after stroke: implications for task-specific approaches to upper-extremity neurorehabilitation. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2013;27:602-612.
21. Jebsen RH, Taylor N, Trieschmann RB, Trotter MJ, Howard LA. An objective and standardized test of hand function. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1969;50:311-319.
22. Mani S, Mutha PK, Przybyla A, Haaland KY, Good DC, Sainburg RL. Contralesional motor deficits after unilateral stroke reflect hemisphere-specific control mechanisms. Brain. 2013;136(pt 4):1288-1303.
23. Schaefer SY, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL. Hemispheric specialization and functional impact of ipsilesional deficits in movement coordination and accuracy. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2953-2966.
24. Cherry KM, Lenze EJ, Lang CE. Combining d-cycloserine with motor training does not result in improved general motor learning in neurologically intact people or in people with stroke. J Neurophysiol. 2014;111:2516-2524.
25. Winstein CJ. Knowledge of results and motor learning–implications for physical therapy. Phys Ther. 1991;71:140-149.
26. Altman DG, Bland JM. How to randomise. BMJ. 1999;319:703-704.
27. Prager EM, Birkenmeier RL, Lang CE. Exploring expectations for upper-extremity motor treatment in people after stroke: a secondary analysis. Am J Occup Ther. 2011;65:437-444.
28. Waddell KJ, Birkenmeier RL, Moore JL, Hornby TG, Lang CE. Feasibility of high-repetition, task-specific training for individuals with upper-extremity paresis. Am J Occup Ther. 2014;68:444-453.
29. Deuschl G, Toro C, Zeffiro T, Massaquoi S, Hallett M. Adaptation motor learning of arm movements in patients with cerebellar disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1996;60:515-519.
30. Lang CE, Bastian AJ. Cerebellar subjects show impaired adaptation of anticipatory EMG during catching. J Neurophysiol. 1999;82:2108-2119.
31. Martin TA, Keating JG, Goodkin HP, Bastian AJ, Thach WT. Throwing while looking through prisms. I. Focal olivocerebellar lesions impair adaptation. Brain. 1996;119(pt 4):1183-1198.
32. Joiner WM, Smith MA. Long-term retention explained by a model of short-term learning in the adaptive control of reaching. J Neurophysiol. 2008;100:2948-2955.
33. Krakauer JW, Shadmehr R. Consolidation of motor memory. Trends Neurosci. 2006;29:58-64.
34. Freitas S, Prieto G, Simoes MR, Santana I. Psychometric properties of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA): an analysis using the Rasch model. Clin Neuropsychol. 2014;28:65-83.
35. Hoops S, Nazem S, Siderowf AD, et al. Validity of the MoCA and MMSE in the detection of MCI and dementia in Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2009;73:1738-1745.
36. Rossetti HC, Lacritz LH, Cullum CM, Weiner MF. Normative data for the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in a population-based sample. Neurology. 2011;77:1272-1275.
37. Suzman RM, Willis DP, Manton KG, eds. The Oldest Old. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1992.
38. Kaye JA, Oken BS, Howieson DB, Howieson J, Holm LA, Dennison K. Neurologic evaluation of the optimally healthy oldest old. Arch Neurol. 1994;51:1205-1211.
39. Bedard P, Sanes JN. Basal ganglia-dependent processes in recalling learned visual-motor adaptations. Exp Brain Res. 2011;209:385-393.
40. Werner S, Bock O, Gizewski ER, Schoch B, Timmann D. Visuomotor adaptive improvement and aftereffects are impaired differentially following cerebellar lesions in SCA and PICA territory. Exp Brain Res. 2010;201:429-439.
41. Werner S, Bock O, Timmann D. The effect of cerebellar cortical degeneration on adaptive plasticity and movement control. Exp Brain Res. 2009;193:189-196.
42. Orrell AJ, Eves FF, Masters RS. Implicit motor learning of a balancing task. Gait Posture. 2006;23:9-16.
43. Kimberley TJ, Samargia S, Moore LG, Shakya JK, Lang CE. Comparison of amounts and types of practice during rehabilitation for traumatic brain injury and stroke. J Rehabil Res Dev. 2010;47:851-862.
44. Lang CE, Macdonald JR, Reisman DS, et al. Observation of amounts of movement practice provided during stroke rehabilitation. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2009;90:1692-1698.
45. Bowden MG, Woodbury ML, Duncan PW. Promoting neuroplasticity and recovery after stroke: future directions for rehabilitation clinical trials. Curr Opin Neurol. 2013;26:37-42.
46. Lohse KR, Lang CE, Boyd LA. Is more better? Using metadata to explore dose-response relationships in stroke rehabilitation. Stroke. 2014;45:2053-2058.
47. Schmidt RA, Lee TD. Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis. 3rd ed. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics; 1999.
48. Abellan van Kan G, Rolland Y, Gillette-Guyonnet S, et al. Gait speed, body composition, and dementia. The EPIDOS-Toulouse cohort. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2012;67:425-432.
49. Fauth EB, Schwartz S, Tschanz JT, Ostbye T, Corcoran C, Norton MC. Baseline disability in activities of daily living predicts dementia risk even after controlling for baseline global cognitive ability and depressive symptoms. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2013;28:597-606.
50. Krakauer JW. Motor learning and consolidation: the case of visuomotor rotation. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2009;629:405-421.
51. Vidoni ED, Boyd LA. Achieving enlightenment: what do we know about the implicit learning system and its interaction with explicit knowledge? J Neurol Phys Ther. 2007;31:145-154.
52. Della-Maggiore V, Scholz J, Johansen-Berg H, Paus T. The rate of visuomotor adaptation correlates with cerebellar white-matter microstructure. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:4048-4053.
53. Duff K. One-week practice effects in older adults: tools for assessing cognitive change. Clin Neuropsychol. 2014;28:714-725.

Cite article

Cite article

Cite article

OR

Download to reference manager

If you have citation software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice

Share options

Share

Share this article

Share with email
Email Article Link
Share on social media

Share access to this article

Sharing links are not relevant where the article is open access and not available if you do not have a subscription.

For more information view the Sage Journals article sharing page.

Information, rights and permissions

Information

Published In

Article first published online: November 21, 2014
Issue published: August 2015

Keywords

  1. aging
  2. upper extremity
  3. task-specific training
  4. motor learning
  5. cognition

Rights and permissions

© The Author(s) 2014.
Request permissions for this article.
PubMed: 25416739

Authors

Affiliations

Sydney Y. Schaefer, PhD
Utah State University, Logan, UT, USA
University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Leland E. Dibble, PhD
University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Kevin Duff, PhD
University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA

Notes

Sydney Y. Schaefer, Department of Health, Physical Education and Recreation, Utah State University, 7000 Old Main Hill, Logan, UT 84322, USA. Email: [email protected]

Metrics and citations

Metrics

Journals metrics

This article was published in Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair.

View All Journal Metrics

Article usage*

Total views and downloads: 1949

*Article usage tracking started in December 2016


Altmetric

See the impact this article is making through the number of times it’s been read, and the Altmetric Score.
Learn more about the Altmetric Scores



Articles citing this one

Receive email alerts when this article is cited

Web of Science: 29 view articles Opens in new tab

Crossref: 31

  1. Rey-Osterrieth complex figure recall scores and motor skill learning in older adults: A non-linear mixed effect model-based analysis
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle Scholar
  2. Mediation Analysis of the Effect of Visuospatial Memory on Motor Skill Learning in Older Adults
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle Scholar
  3. Improving Prediction of Amyloid Deposition in Mild Cognitive Impairment With a Timed Motor Task
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle ScholarPub Med
  4. Resting EEG spectral slopes are associated with age-related differences in information processing speed
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle Scholar
  5. Delays in the Reticulospinal System Are Associated With a Reduced Capacity to Learn a Simulated Feeding Task in Older Adults
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle Scholar
  6. Association Between Motor Task Performance and Hippocampal Atrophy Across Cognitively Unimpaired, Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment, and Alzheimer’s Disease Individuals
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle ScholarPub Med
  7. Relating Global Cognition With Upper-Extremity Motor Skill Retention in Individuals With Mild-to-Moderate Parkinson's Disease
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle Scholar
  8. Remote, Unsupervised Functional Motor Task Evaluation in Older Adults across the United States Using the MindCrowd Electronic Cohort
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle Scholar
  9. How Common Is the Exponential Decay Pattern of Motor Skill Acquisition? A Brief Investigation
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle Scholar
  10. Age-related differences in functional tool-use are due to changes in movement quality and not simply motor slowing
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle Scholar
  11. View More

Figures and tables

Figures & Media

Tables

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Access options

If you have access to journal content via a personal subscription, university, library, employer or society, select from the options below:

ASNR Member Access


Members must activate their accounts to access the journal. Please read the instructions on activating your account or signing in to an existing account for help.

ASNR Member Access


Members must activate their accounts to access the journal. Please read the instructions on activating your account or signing in to an existing account for help.


Alternatively, view purchase options below:

Purchase 24 hour online access to view and download content.

Access journal content via a DeepDyve subscription or find out more about this option.