Skip to main content

[]

Intended for healthcare professionals
Skip to main content
Open access
Research article
First published online February 22, 2023

Longitudinal Stability of Disordered-Eating Symptoms From Age 12 to 40 in Black and White Women

Abstract

The purpose of the current study was to test the longitudinal association between disordered-eating symptoms (body dissatisfaction, drive for thinness, and bulimia) in adolescence (ages 12, 14, 16, 18, 19) and adulthood (age 40) in a sample of 883 white and Black women. We also investigated moderation by race. Adolescent symptoms at each time point significantly predicted adulthood symptoms for the Body Dissatisfaction and Drive for Thinness subscales for both Black and white women. Bulimia symptoms in adolescence predicted symptoms in adulthood; however, the effect was largely driven by white women. Although moderation was nonsignificant, among white women, bulimia symptoms at all adolescent time points predicted adulthood bulimia, but among Black women, only symptoms at ages 18 and 19 were predictive of adulthood bulimia. Results suggest that both Black and white women are susceptible to disordered eating and that symptoms emerging in adolescence can potentially follow women into midlife.
Eating disorders and their symptoms are associated with myriad mental- and physical-health consequences, including cardiovascular disease (Casiero & Frishman, 2006), neurological deficits (Johnson et al., 2002), suicidal ideation (Neumark-Sztainer et al., 1998), substance use (Crow et al., 2006), and premature mortality (Keel et al., 2003). Symptoms of eating disorders often begin in adolescence, concurrent with puberty, during which young girls become increasingly vigilant of their changing body shape and size (Kelch-Oliver & Ancis, 2011; Voelker et al., 2015). Meta-analyses have indicated that although lifetime prevalence of clinical eating disorders is only 8% among women, more than 60% of adolescent girls present with subclinical disordered-eating behaviors and cognitions (Bartholdy et al., 2017). Accordingly, studies demonstrate that the onset of symptoms occurs long before a clinical diagnosis and may persist throughout the life span (McClelland et al., 2020; Stice et al., 2011; Stice & Van Ryzin, 2019). For example, one study of elementary and middle school age children illustrated short-term stability of symptoms—children who reported restricting or bingeing food in fifth grade were about 3 and 6 times more likely to report those same behaviors, respectively, 1 year later (Combs et al., 2013). Kotler et al. (2001) provided evidence for longer-term stability of eating-disorder symptoms—girls that exhibited disordered-eating symptoms in early adolescence had approximately 2 to 3 times greater odds of experiencing those symptoms 17 years later. Nonetheless, questions remain regarding how stable disordered-eating symptoms are across longer periods of time or if a specific age of onset in adolescence is particularly influential for stability of symptoms into adulthood.
Moreover, recent dialogue has elucidated the extent to which racial and ethnic minorities remain underrepresented in disordered-eating research—a meta-analysis demonstrated that fewer than half of studies report race/ethnicity data, and of those that do, white participants make up 70% of research samples (Egbert et al., 2022; Sonneville & Lipson, 2018). Although Black women make up fewer than 5% of the research population (Egbert et al., 2022), previous research suggests that they demonstrate patterns of disordered eating distinct from white women. Indeed, eating disorders and embodiment occur in a sociocultural context, one that specifically includes racial and cultural influences on eating, beauty, body shape, and body size (Watson et al., 2019). For example, perhaps because of cultural norms and differences in idealized body shape and size (White & Grilo, 2005), Black women may be less vulnerable to thin standards of beauty, instead favoring a fuller-figured body (Capodilupo & Kim, 2014; Rucker & Cash, 1992). Several studies have asserted that Black female college students report fewer body-image concerns (Quick & Byrd-Bredbenner, 2014) and less body-image distortion than white students (Gluck & Geliebter, 2002); however, from middle school through young adulthood, Black girls report a steeper increase in body dissatisfaction over time than their white counterparts (Bucchianeri et al., 2013). Potential differences in bulimic symptoms for white and Black women also exist. Whereas some studies have asserted that white women report more bulimic symptoms than Black women (Perez & Joiner, 2003; Striegel-Moore et al., 2003), across ages 11 to 14, Black girls score higher than white girls on the Bulimia subscale of the Eating Disorder Inventory (EDI; Striegel-Moore, Schreiber, et al., 2000). Thus, although the fuller-figured, Afrocentric idealized body shape and size may be protective against body dissatisfaction and disordered eating to an extent, Black women and girls are not entirely immune from the dominant, white-centric cultural influences that idealize thinness and the associated patterns of eating behaviors women may engage in to achieve thinner silhouettes (Williamson, 1998).
Observed discrepancies in incidence and prevalence of disordered eating and body dissatisfaction between racial and ethnic groups may also exist because they have been primarily assessed through a white and Western lens (Wildes et al., 2001). Lower levels of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating reported among Black women may have more to do with overreliance on white-centric conceptualizations of beauty and the inability of existing measures to account for racially salient influences on disordered eating and notions of beauty (Cassidy et al., 2015; Lowy et al., 2021). Disordered-eating behaviors and cognitions among Black and white women are informed by distinct conceptualizations of femininity, and both racial and gender identities contribute to differences in how women think and feel about their bodies (Lovejoy, 2001). This is best contextualized through the lens of objectification theory, which holds that in an oppressive society, women are often cultured to view their bodies through the lens of the oppressor (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997; Schaefer et al., 2018). Although for white women, sexism undoubtedly influences their eating behaviors and how they feel about their bodies, Black women also contend with racial discrimination. The multiplicative effects of racism and sexism on disordered eating and embodiment have not been extensively investigated (Watson et al., 2012, 2019). Disordered eating may not only be a means to control body shape and size but also a coping strategy and a locus of control in oppressive social contexts (Kempa & Thomas, 2000). Thus, in addition to examining disordered-eating trends across longer time spans, specific care should be taken to examine the patterns of disordered-eating symptoms in different racial groups and, specifically, how the trajectory of these symptoms may change over time, differentially.

The Current Study

It is imperative to understand the stability of eating behaviors in childhood and adolescence because they are established risk factors for disordered-eating symptoms and clinical disorders in adulthood alongside adverse health outcomes. Although some studies have demonstrated stability of disordered-eating symptoms across childhood and adolescence, longer-term effects in middle adulthood are still unknown. To our knowledge, the current analysis is the longest test of the association between disordered-eating behavior and cognitions in adolescence (ages 12–19) and those same behaviors and cognitions in adulthood (age 40). Moreover, in the present study, we focused on a diverse sample of girls to provide insight into the trajectories of eating-disorder symptoms in adolescent and adult Black women. We hypothesize that disordered-eating symptoms across ages 12 to 19 will predict disordered-eating symptoms at age 40. We expect that the above relationship will be moderated by race for drive for thinness and bulimia such that the longitudinal association between disordered eating in adolescence and adulthood will be stronger in white women compared with Black women because Black women are potentially less susceptible to the Western thin ideal (Ordaz et al., 2018; Rucker & Cash, 1992; Striegel-Moore et al., 2003). Without a clear consensus from existing literature on body dissatisfaction in Black and white women, race moderation for this subscale was treated as exploratory (i.e., we did not propose a directional hypothesis).

Method

Author positionality

We and others (e.g., Cole, 2020; Roberts et al., 2020) acknowledge the importance of asserting positionality, or defining the individual lenses through which we as researchers approach a given study or program of research because our identities may influence how we conceptualize, report, and write about research findings. Eating-disorders literature has been disproportionately focused on and conducted by cisgender white women, and far fewer studies have acknowledged the prevalence of these behaviors among minority women or approached this field of work through a culturally salient lens (Halbeisen et al., 2022). Thus, we feel it is important to offer transparency through which this group of authors approached the present study. J. E. Parker identifies as a millennial, upper-middle-class, cisgender, Black woman and is a psychology doctoral student who studies body image, eating behaviors, and health in Black women. J. A. Levinson identifies as a middle-class, cisgender, white female doctoral student in psychology who studies contributors to and consequences of weight stigma and disordered-eating behaviors and cognitions in adolescence and throughout the life span. J. M. Hunger identifies as a white, upper-middle-class, cisgender gay man who is trained as a social/health psychologist with particular expertise on the health effects of stigma and discrimination. C. K. Enders identifies as a white male quantitative psychologist whose research expertise centers on missing data methodologies. B. A. Laraia is a senior researcher who is a cisgender, white, upper-middle-class female with expertise in the measurement and analysis of dietary intake, food security, and eating behaviors. E. S. Epel is a midlife, upper-middle-class, white woman with a background in health psychology and health disparities. A. J. Tomiyama, the senior author, identifies as an Asian American, upper-middle-class, cisgender woman who is a social-health psychologist with interests in weight stigma, body image, stress, and eating behavior.

Study design

Data for the present study were collected as part of the National Heart Lung and Blood Institute’s Growth and Health Study, a longitudinal cohort study spanning adolescence (ages 10–19) and adulthood (age 40) in a diverse sample of women. The primary goal of the study was to identify psychosocial factors associated with the development of obesity, including disordered-eating behaviors, cognitions, and pathology. The study began in 1987, enrolling Black and white girls between the ages of 9 and 10 across three primary recruitment sites in Richmond, California, Washington, D.C., and Cincinnati, Ohio. The women included in the age-40 follow-up time point were from the Richmond site only. Additional details about the study and participant cohort have been extensively described elsewhere (The National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute Growth and Health Study & Research Group, 1992).

Participants

Baseline enrollment period for the study spanned from 1987 to 1989, during which investigators enrolled 2,379 girls (1,213 Black and 1,166 white). Inclusion criteria were current age within 2 weeks younger or older than 9 or 10 years old, respectively; self-reported race of white or Black; and living with a parent or guardian of the same self-reported race who was willing to consent on behalf of the child and provide demographic information about the household and family. Across 10 annual time points (≈10–19 years old), data were collected via in-home visits, during which a trained study staff member administered psychosocial and demographic questionnaires and collected anthropometric measurements.
Of the 2,379 girls who completed the initial clinical visit and the subset of 883 girls who were from the Richmond site, 624 of those women completed a follow-up visit at approximately age 40. After consent for the follow-up study, psychosocial data on stress and eating behaviors were collected via mailed survey. Study staff then scheduled an in-home visit to obtain anthropometric measurements.
We initially preregistered a sample size of 624; however, this decision would have assumed that the data missing from 259 women who dropped out between adolescent and follow-up data were due to a haphazard missing completely at random process. To avoid this assumption, we ran analyses on both sets of data (i.e., with and without the 259 women included, rendering samples sizes of 883 and 624, respectively) and determined that the analyses yielded identical inferences. Thus, the analyses presented below correspond to the full sample of 883, which leverages the more realistic, conditionally missing at random assumption. The parallel analyses for the smaller sample are presented in the Supplemental Material available online.

Measures

The focal predictors and outcomes in adolescence and adulthood were the Body Dissatisfaction (BD), Drive for Thinness (DT), and Bulimia subscales of the EDI, Versions 1 (EDI-1) and 3 (EDI-3), respectively (Garner, 2004; Garner et al., 1983). Studies that have investigated the psychometric properties of the various EDI subscales suggest that all three are valid, reliable, temporally stable, and internally consistent in both Black and white women (Bardone-Cone & Boyd, 2007; Kelly et al., 2012; Rothstein et al., 2017; Wilfley et al., 1996). The DT and Bulimia subscales appear to be factor invariant between Black and white women (Rothstein et al., 2017); however, more recent work suggests that the BD subscale captures some but not all of Black women’s body-image concerns (Parker et al., 2022). Although more culturally salient measures may more holistically represent Black women’s body-image concerns (Wilfred & Lundgren, 2021), the BD subscale nonetheless is reliable and valid for measuring the intended body-image concerns (Kashubeck-West et al., 2013). In the present study, this measure can be thought of as indexing the discrepancy between Black women’s perceived and idealized body, which reliably predicts their BD scores (Gordon et al., 2010).

EDI-1

The focal predictors in adolescence were the BD, DT, and Bulimia subscales of the EDI-1 (Garner et al., 1983). The EDI-1 was administered at ages 12, 14, 16, 18, and 19, resulting in five measurement time points for each subscale (measurement time points were dummy coded to account for unequal intervals). A brief overview of each subscale is as follows: The BD subscale contains nine items that represent preoccupation with one’s own shape and size (e.g., “I feel satisfied with the shape of my body,” “I think that my thighs are too big,” and “I like the shape of my behind”), the DT subscale contains seven items that reflect excessive concern with dieting and weight and a pursuit of a thinner figure (e.g., “I feel very guilty after I eat too much,” “I care too much about my weight,” and “I think about being thinner a lot”), and the Bulimia subscale contains seven items that index tendency for uncontrollable eating (bingeing) and associated compensatory behaviors (e.g., “I eat when I am upset,” “Sometimes I have started eating too much and could not stop,” and “I think about trying to throw up to lose weight”). Individual items in each subscale are rated on a 6-point scale, corresponding to always, usually, often, sometimes, rarely, or never. The first three response options on the scale are coded 3, 2, and 1, respectively, and the latter three options all receive a score of 0. The resultant score ranges for the DT, BD, and Bulimia subscales are 0 to 21, 0 to 27, and 0 to 21, respectively.

EDI-3

At age 40, participants again completed the EDI, albeit an updated version of the questionnaire (EDI-3; Garner, 2004). The three subscales were scored on the same 6-point rating scale; however, responses were coded from 0 to 4. Always, usually, often, and sometimes received respective scores of 4, 3, 2, and 1, and both rarely and never received scores of 0. The BD subscale contains 10 items with a score range of 0 to 40 (Cronbach’s αs = .86 overall, .88 for white women, and .83 for Black women), the Drive for Thinness subscale contains seven items with a score range of 0 to 28 (Cronbach’s αs = .86 overall, .89 for white women, and .83 for Black women), and the Bulimia subscale contains eight items with a score range of 0 to 32 (Cronbach’s αs = .86 overall, .90 for white women, and .81 for Black women). Despite minor changes in scoring, subscales index the same disordered-eating behaviors.
We additionally conducted confirmatory factor analyses using our own data to test configural and measurement invariance for each EDI-3 subscale in adulthood across Black and white women (Gregorich, 2006). The Bulimia subscale demonstrated sufficient configural and measurement invariance across race (comparative fix index [CFI] = .95, root mean square error of approximation [RMSEA] = .10, 90% confidence interval [CI] = [.09, .11]), χ2(9) = 53.75, p < .001. The BD and DT subscales had sufficient configural invariance as measured by the CFI but not the RMSEA (BD: CFI = .95, RMSEA = .10, 90% CI = [.09, .11]; DT: CFI = 1.00, RMSEA = .008, 90% CI = [.00, .05]). Neither subscale demonstrated metric invariance: BD, χ2(6) = 18.61, p = .005; DT, χ2(7) = 10.31, p = .17. This pattern is largely consistent with previous studies.

Socioeconomic status

Parental income and education were included as covariates (dummy coded) in all analyses and were collected at baseline, when participants were first enrolled in the study in adolescence (age 10). We elected to use a measure of socioeconomic status (SES) in adolescence as opposed to an equivalent measure in adulthood because adolescent SES could plausibly influence both our adolescent and adulthood variables, whereas adult SES could not retrospectively cause adolescent outcomes. Parental income was measured on a 4-point categorical scale defined as (1) less than $5,000, (2) $5,000 to $20,000, (3) $20,000 to $40,000, and (4) $40,000 or more. Parental education was defined as the highest level of education obtained by the participant’s primary caregiver on a 3-point categorical scale: (1) high school or less, (2) 1 to 3 years of education after high school, and (3) 4-year college degree or more.

Analytic plan

Model construction

We fit three multiple linear regression models (one per EDI subscale) to examine the relationship between disordered-eating symptoms in adolescence and disordered-eating symptoms at age 40, test moderation by race, and examine the pattern of age-specific effects. Missing data were addressed using a Bayesian model-based imputation procedure. Further details are provided in the Supplemental Material.
Outlier detection was performed in accordance with the preregistration. For each model, potential influential points were identified by visualizing a plot of model residuals versus leverage, examining the DFBETAS for each participant (i.e., comparing the differences in parameter estimates with and without the inclusion of a given observation), and estimating models with and without any potential influential point or points. In addition, each model was run with and without the inclusion of cases with standardized residuals greater than an absolute value of 3 (i.e., ±3 SD). Inference was determined to be robust to inclusion/exclusion of data points.

Hypotheses

To test whether disordered-eating symptoms throughout adolescence predicted disordered-eating symptoms at age 40 (Hypothesis 1), we examined the two-way interaction terms between disordered-eating symptoms and age (dummy coded). To test whether the age-specific associations were moderated by race (Hypothesis 2), we additionally included three-way interaction terms between race, disordered-eating symptoms, and age.
The three-way interactions reflected whether the age-specific influence of disordered eating (i.e., the two-way interaction) differed by race. If the omnibus test of moderation (i.e., the test of significance for the four three-way interaction terms between race, disordered eating, and the dummy-coded age variables) for Hypothesis 2 was significant, we used contrasts to examine the pattern of age-specific effects in each racial group. If the omnibus test of the three-way interaction terms was nonsignificant, the terms were dropped from the model (along with the two-way interaction terms between race and age and race and disordered eating), and we examined the lower-order two-way interactions between disordered eating and age, averaged across race. Given that literature examining disordered-eating behaviors longitudinally among Black women is limited and the call for more inclusive eating-disorders research (Egbert et al., 2022; Lowy et al., 2021; Mikhail & Klump, 2021; Rodgers et al., 2018), we diverge from our preregistration to report the race- and age-specific effects (i.e., the age-specific effects for both white and Black women) regardless of significance of the three-way interaction terms. To control Type I error, we used the false-discovery-rate approach (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995) with a false positive rate set at 5% per model, as stated in our preregistration.

Preregistration

All analyses were preregistered at https://osf.io/6dws2/.

Results

Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics for Black (n = 455) and white women (n = 428), stratified by age. Means observed for the three EDI-1 and EDI-3 subscales largely appeared to be consistent with published community norms based on age- and race-specific patterns (Clausen et al., 2011; Gordon et al., 2010; Kelly et al., 2012; Klemchuk et al., 1990; Nyman-Carlsson et al., 2015; Rancourt et al., 2022; Rosen et al., 1988; Rothstein et al., 2017). Specific details on comparable norms for each subscale, stratified by version and with age- and race-specific reference groups, can be found in the Supplemental Material. We also report on the percentage of participants who scored above a zero on each measure, stratified by race, subscale, and age (see Table S1 in the Supplemental Material). In addition, the Eating Disorder Risk Composite (EDRC; Garner et al., 1983) score—a global measure of eating concerns—was calculated at age 40 using the EDI-3 and is presented in Table S2 in the Supplemental Material. Because the National Heart Lung and Blood Institute group did not collect clinical diagnoses, we present the EDRC scores and clinical qualitative ranges with both anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa as the reference groups. More than 30% of our sample of Black women and 50% of our sample of white women appeared to be at typical or elevated clinical risk for anorexia nervosa. Approximately 2% of Black women and 8% of white women had typical or elevated risk for bulimia nervosa.
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for Participants, Stratified by Race, Across All Time Points
 Age 10Age 12Age 14Age 16Age 18Age 19Age 40
Black women (n = 455)       
Parental education (%)       
≤ High school26.03%      
1–3 years of education after high school54.40%      
≥ 4-year college degree19.57%      
Parental income (%)       
< $5,00033.77%      
$5,000–$20,00022.81%      
$20,000–$40,00023.84%      
≥ $40,00016.58%      
Body Dissatisfaction subscale 6.29 (6.40)6.23 (6.44)6.20 (6.45)7.36 (7.21)7.87 (7.17)19.25 (8.68)
Drive for Thinness subscale 4.49 (5.08)3.63 (4.97)3.62 (4.98)3.56 (4.91)4.10 (5.17)8.33 (6.86)
Bulimia subscale 2.41 (3.78)1.96 (3.28)1.27 (2.38)0.92 (2.03)0.92 (2.12)2.04 (3.94)
White women (n = 428)       
Parental education (%)       
≤ High school20.92%      
1–3 years of education after high school39.57%      
≥ 4-year college degree39.51%      
Parental income (%)       
< $5,0007.55%      
$5,000–$20,00012.15%      
$20,000–$40,00034.49%      
≥ $40,00045.80%      
Body Dissatisfaction subscale 7.47 (7.42)8.52 (7.37)8.50 (7.39)10.36 (8.36)10.60 (8.14)22.89 (9.12)
Drive for Thinness subscale 4.85 (5.81)4.92 (5.89)4.90 (5.92)5.49 (6.12)5.80 (6.10)10.45 (7.71)
Bulimia subscale 1.39 (2.95)1.37 (2.81)1.09 (2.58)0.95 (2.48)0.91 (2.35)3.24 (4.59)
Note: Values presented in the table for parental income and education are the percentage of the sample of Black or White women represented in that category. Values presented for the Body Dissatisfaction, Drive for Thinness, and Bulimia subscales are means and standard deviations (in parentheses).

Body dissatisfaction

The association between body dissatisfaction in adolescence and adulthood was not moderated by race, F(4, 276.88) = 1.22, p = .43, nor was there evidence for moderation by race at any time point (see Table S3 in the Supplemental Material). Thus, the three-way interaction was dropped from the model, and we examined the two-way interaction between age and body dissatisfaction averaged across race, which was significant, F(4, 410.03) = 3.75, p = .01.
Table 2 presents the results for the model predicting the association between adolescent and adulthood body dissatisfaction, averaged across racial groups. The estimate for the β1 coefficient represents the conditional effect of body dissatisfaction at age 12 on body dissatisfaction at age 40, averaged across Black and white women. At age 12, body dissatisfaction significantly predicted adulthood body dissatisfaction such that increases in body dissatisfaction at age 12 predicted greater body dissatisfaction at age 40, controlling for parental income and education (β1 = 0.32, p < .001). To determine the remaining age-specific associations, we estimated the conditional effects at each time point, which are presented at the bottom of Table 2. At ages 14, 16, 18, and 19, increases in body dissatisfaction in adolescence predicted later increases in body dissatisfaction in adulthood (all ps < .001). Note that the effects were noticeably stronger at ages 18 and 19.
Table 2. Body Dissatisfaction Model Results Without Moderation (Hypothesis 1)
ParameterEstimateSEp
β021.200.69< .001
β1 (body dissatisfaction)0.320.05< .001
Age   
Age 12Reference group
β2 (Age 14)−0.310.63.73
β3 (Age 16)−0.360.68.71
β4 (Age 18)−1.860.64.01
β5 (Age 19)−2.380.65.001
Race   
WhiteReference group
β6 (Black)−2.800.32< .001
Parental education   
(≤ High school)Reference group
β7 (1–3 years of education after high school)−1.540.45.003
β8 (≥ 4-year college degree)−2.500.50< .001
Parental income   
< $5,000Reference group
β9 ($5,000–$20,000)1.230.58.07
β10 ($20,000–$40,000)−0.210.52.75
β11 (≥ $40,000)1.010.50.08
Body Dissatisfaction × Age interactions   
Body Dissatisfaction × Age 12Reference group
β12 (Body Dissatisfaction × Age 14)0.020.07.81
β13 (Body Dissatisfaction × Age 16)0.030.07.75
β14 (Body Dissatisfaction × Age 18)0.150.07.05
β15 (Body Dissatisfaction × Age 19)0.190.06.01
 Conditional effects by age
Body dissatisfaction at 140.340.05< .001
Body dissatisfaction at 160.350.05< .001
Body dissatisfaction at 180.470.04< .001
Body dissatisfaction at 190.510.04< .001
Whereas the conditional effects in Table 2 represent the average association at each time point (e.g., Is the association between adolescent and adulthood body dissatisfaction significant across Black and white women?), we also wanted to determine whether the conditional effects for each racial group were significant at each age (e.g., Is the association between body dissatisfaction in adolescence in adulthood significant for Black women? Is it significant for white women?). Thus, as exploratory analyses, we additionally estimated these conditional effects stratified by racial group to examine the pattern of age-specific effects for white and Black women separately (see Table S4 in the Supplemental Material). At each age, body dissatisfaction in adolescence significantly predicted body dissatisfaction in adulthood for both Black and white women (all ps < .01). The observed effects were similar in size and magnitude, which is consistent with the absence of a significant three-way interaction in which the association between age and body dissatisfaction was moderated by race.

Drive for thinness

The overall association between drive for thinness in adolescence and adulthood was not moderated by race, F(4, 373.34) = 0.16, p = .96, and there was no evidence of moderation by race at any time point (see Table S5 in the Supplemental Material). Thus, the three-way interaction terms were dropped from the model, and it was subsequently reestimated to examine the age-specific associations averaged across racial groups. The two-way interaction between age and drive for thinness was significant, F(4, 256.06) = 3.73, p = .01.
Table 3 presents the age-specific associations averaged across Black and white women. The estimate for the β1 coefficient represents the conditional effect of drive for thinness at age 12 on drive for thinness at age 40 averaged across Black and white women. At age 12, drive for thinness in adolescence significantly predicted drive for thinness in adulthood such that increases in drive for thinness in adolescence predicted greater drive for thinness at age 40, controlling for parental income and education (β1 = 0.33, p < .001). To determine the remaining age-specific associations, we estimated the conditional effects at each time point, which are presented at the bottom of Table 3—at ages 14, 16, 18, and 19, drive for thinness in adolescence remained a significant predictor of adulthood drive for thinness, controlling for covariates, such that increases in drive for thinness in adolescence were associated with greater drive for thinness in adulthood (all ps < .001).
Table 3. Drive for Thinness Model Results Without Moderation (Hypothesis 1)
ParameterEstimateSEp
b09.190.52< .001
β1 (drive for thinness)0.330.06< .001
Age   
Age 12Reference group
β2 (Age 14)−0.220.51.75
β3 (Age 16)−0.220.49.75
β4 (Age 18)−0.960.52.13
β5 (Age 19)−1.200.49.03
Race   
WhiteReference group
β6 (Black)−1.920.27< .001
Parental education   
≤ High schoolReference group
β7 (1–3 years of education after high school)0.730.31.04
β8 (≥ 4-year college degree)−0.490.35.29
Parental income   
< $5,000Reference group
β9 ($5,000–$20,000)−0.270.41.68
β10 ($20,000–$40,000)−1.120.39.01
β11 (≥ $40,000)−0.590.39.23
Drive for Thinness × Age interactions   
Drive for Thinness × Age 12Reference group
β12 (Drive for Thinness × Age 14)0.070.08.60
β13 (Drive for Thinness × Age 16)0.070.09.60
β14 (Drive for Thinness × Age 18)0.220.08.02
β15 (Drive for Thinness × Age 19)0.240.07.002
 Conditional effects by age
Drive for thinness at 140.400.05< .001
Drive for thinness at 160.400.06< .001
Drive for thinness at 180.550.05< .001
Drive for thinness at 190.570.05< .001
Again, as exploratory analyses, we calculated the conditional effects stratified by racial group to examine the pattern of age-specific effects in each racial group (see Table S6 in the Supplemental Material). For both Black and white women, drive for thinness across ages 12, 14, 16, 18, and 19 predicted drive for thinness at age 40 such that drive for thinness in adolescence was positively associated with greater drive for thinness in adulthood (all ps < .01). Effects were again quite similar between racial groups, which is consistent with the absence of a three-way interaction effect in which race moderated the two-way association between age and drive for thinness.

Bulimia

The omnibus test of moderation by race was nonsignificant for the Bulimia subscale, F(4, 278.81) = 0.49, p = .78. Although age-specific trends for moderation were evident (i.e., at ages 16 and 19), these effects did not survive correction for multiple comparisons (see Table S7 in the Supplemental Material). After dropping the three-way interaction terms between race, bulimia symptoms, and age from the model, the remaining two-way interaction between bulimia symptoms and age was significant, F(4, 324.33) = 15.39, p < .001.
Table 4 presents the results for the model predicting adulthood bulimia symptoms from adolescent bulimia symptoms averaged across racial groups. Results indicated that at all ages, bulimia symptoms in adolescence predicted bulimia symptoms in adulthood, and greater adolescent symptoms positively predicted symptoms in adulthood (Table 4). The β1 coefficient represents the conditional effect of bulimia symptoms at age 12 on bulimia at age 40 averaged across Black and white women. To determine the remaining age-specific associations, we estimated the conditional effects at each age, which are presented at the bottom of Table 4. At age 12, the conditional effect was only marginally significant (p = .05), whereas for ages 14, 16, 18, and 19, the effect was statistically significant.
Table 4. Bulimia Model Results Without Moderation (Hypothesis 1)
ParameterEstimateSEp
β03.400.31< .001
β1 (bulimia)0.120.05.05
Age   
Age 12Reference group
β2 (Age 14)−0.080.28.78
β3 (Age 16)−0.150.28.68
β4 (Age 18)−0.420.26.24
β5 (Age 19)−0.300.28.39
Race   
WhiteReference group
β6 (Black)−1.520.17< .001
Parental education   
(≤ High school)Reference group
β7 (1-3 years of education after high school)0.250.21.37
β8 (≥ 4-year college degree)−0.330.25.31
Parental income   
(< $5,000)Reference group
β9 ($5,000–$20,000)0.150.28.68
β10 ($20,000–$40,000)−0.530.26.10
β11 (≥ $40,000)−0.370.27.30
Bulimia × Age interactions   
Bulimia × Age 12Reference group
β12 (Bulimia × Age 14)0.050.07.63
β13 (Bulimia × Age 16)0.200.09.09
β14 (Bulimia × Age 18)0.590.09< .001
β15 (Bulimia × Age 19)0.480.09< .001
 Conditional effects by age
Bulimia at 140.170.05.01
Bulimia at 160.320.07.01
Bulimia at 180.720.08< .001
Bulimia at 190.600.08< .001
Finally, as exploratory analyses, we examined the age-specific effects for white and Black women separately at each time point. For white women, bulimia symptoms at all adolescent time points predicted bulimia symptoms in adulthood; however, for Black women, only bulimia symptoms at ages 18 and 19 significantly predicted adulthood bulimia symptoms (see Table S8 in the Supplemental Material).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this 28-year study represents the largest time gap in which the longitudinal stability of disordered-eating behaviors has been examined. Moreover, this investigation was conducted in a racially diverse sample of 455 Black and 428 white women, advancing research equity in a field in which some estimates show only 5% of participants have been Black women. We hypothesized that adolescent disordered-eating behaviors and cognitions would predict the presence of these same behaviors in adulthood. Furthermore, on the basis of previous literature, we hypothesized that these trends would be distinct between white and Black women. To some extent, our results were in line with the hypotheses: For body dissatisfaction and drive for thinness, adolescent disordered-eating behaviors and cognitions predicted adulthood behaviors and cognitions for both white and Black women, and this effect was significant at all time points. When examining this trend for the Bulimia subscale, the same trend appears to occur—when averaged across racial groups, bulimia symptoms at all adolescent time points predicted bulimia symptoms in adulthood; however, closer inspection of the race-specific effects reveals that this effect is largely driven by white women. Statistical moderation by race was not evident for any of the three disordered-eating behaviors.
Although effects for body dissatisfaction between white and Black women did not differ significantly, we acknowledge that the estimates obtained for Black women may not be adequately capturing all dimensions of body satisfaction. Studies suggest that Black women conceptualize body image differently from their white counterparts (Davis et al., 2010), and indeed, the BD subscale does not adequately capture all body-image concerns for Black women (Kashubeck-West et al., 2013). Using confirmatory factory analyses, Kashubeck-West and colleagues (2013) found that although the BD subscale was reliable and valid among Black women, the measure does not index racially salient body-image concerns for Black women (e.g., hair, lips, skin color). Indeed, along with others, our own work demonstrates that Black women have unique body-image concerns, such as skin color, that are related to their disordered-eating behaviors (Capodilupo & Kim, 2014; Parker et al., 2022). Given the downstream health consequences of body dissatisfaction, future research should measure and examine body image through a lens that acknowledges the sociocultural context in which women evaluate their bodies (Mucherah & Frazier, 2013).
Previously, it was widely accepted that Black women were protected from thin idealization because of cultural norms that favor fuller-figured bodies (Williamson, 1998). Although it is reasonable and there is evidence to suggest that Black women do not value thinness as highly as white women do (e.g., Gluck & Geliebter, 2002), this does not suggest that Black women are then immune from the mainstream societal norms that throughout history have sought to negate their physical qualities as representative of beauty (Hughes, 2021). Our results support this idea: First, no significant differences in drive for thinness emerged between Black and white women, and for both groups, drive for thinness in adolescence influences adulthood drive for thinness; however, examining the magnitude of these estimates suggests that Black women may value thinness to a lesser degree.
Unlike research on the BD subscale of the EDI, evidence suggests that the Bulimia subscale functions as intended among Black women (Bardone-Cone & Boyd, 2007). However, research estimating the prevalence of bulimia among white versus Black women has been conflicting—for example, some studies have estimated a higher prevalence of bulimia diagnoses among white women (Striegel-Moore et al., 2003), and others have found equivalent prevalence when comparing bulimia symptoms (Cachelin et al., 2000). Despite this, pervasive stereotypes of bulimia being a white, affluent, thin woman’s disease have prevailed, resulting in lower rates of treatment and fewer resources for people who fall outside of that stereotype (Sonneville & Lipson, 2018). Deeper exploration of the etiology and symptoms of bulimia among Black women is particularly important in the context of binge-eating disorder, which only recently received diagnostic recognition in the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), and has substantial overlap with bulimia. Binge-eating disorder, in which Black women experience high rates (Goode et al., 2020; Striegel-Moore, Wilfley, et al., 2000; Taylor et al., 2013), is distinct only from bulimia in the use of compensatory behaviors (e.g., purging). Because our data did not capture actual eating-disorder diagnosis, the endorsement of bulimia symptoms likely has overlap with binge-eating disorder. With white and Black women idealizing different body shapes and sizes, bulimic behaviors among white women may coincide with the drive for thinness, but for Black women, perhaps these symptoms could also be related to binge eating or the idealization of a curvier, hourglass figure (Wilfred & Lundgren, 2021).
The relationship between adolescent and adulthood bulimia appeared to emerge at a later age for Black women (ages 18 and 19) than for white women. From a theoretical standpoint, there are myriad psychological, cultural, and familial factors that may explain the apparent later onset (Brytek-Matera & Czepczor, 2017). In the domain of cultural and psychological influence, it may be that as Black girls enter adulthood, they experience greater pressures toward acculturation, leading to idealization of thinner figures and subsequent engagement in bulimic behaviors to achieve said silhouette. There may also be familial, socioeconomic influence on the age-specific presence of bulimic behaviors for Black girls. For example, because of residential segregation and economic disenfranchisement, Black women are the most likely to live in high-density homes, with a greater number of family members living under the same roof (Chambers et al., 2010). The resulting home density may prevent Black girls from being able to secretively engage in some common bulimic behaviors, such as purging, while living at home. Thus, future eating-disorders-intervention research may benefit from considering age-specific and race-specific influences on the onset of bulimic behaviors.

Limitations

Several limitations of the current study should be considered when interpreting the results. From a methodological standpoint, we acknowledge constraints specifically related to our use of the EDI subscale measures. Although at age 40 we had sufficient item-level data to calculate psychometrics for the EDI-3 stratified by subscale and race, we did not have item-level data for the EDI-1 in adolescence to include psychometrics for this particular measure. The absence of item-level data in adolescence unfortunately also precludes examination of how the EDI factor structure, psychometric properties, and change patterns across time may vary between Black and white women in our sample. Given that these measures were specifically developed in and for white women, it would be useful for future longitudinal studies to investigate whether this measure retains equivalent validity, reliability, and stability in diverse groups over time. Note that the Cronbach’s αs for all three EDI-3 subscales were lower for Black women compared with white women in our sample, and only the Bulimia subscale demonstrated configural and measurement invariance. The BD and DT subscales showed evidence for configural invariance only. This pattern of results is largely consistent with previous studies, and for this reason, we suggest that greater confidence should be put in the analyses stratified by race rather than those that compared the EDI measures. The disordered-eating literature may benefit from updated measures that better capture these behaviors across diverse groups, which may include the addition of questions to examine culturally salient disordered-eating behaviors.
Although some studies have indeed demonstrated the validity of the EDI in racial and ethnic minorities, others have questioned whether it fully captures the same constructs in Black and white women (Kashubeck-West et al., 2013). Moreover, the Bulimia subscale of the EDI does not disentangle overlapping symptoms with binge-eating disorder, which accounts for the largest burden of disease among all eating disorders (Santomauro et al., 2021) and across racial groups (Striegel-Moore, Wilfley, et al., 2000). With regard to demographic methodology, a categorical assessment of race did not allow for more nuanced examination of how race as a multidimensional, socially constructed category influences eating behaviors, given studies that suggest the importance of capturing the influence of racial identity rather than dichotomized racial categories (Roth, 2016). In fact, in the introduction to a recently released guidebook for eating-disorder practitioners, titled Treating Black Women With Eating Disorders: A Clinician’s Guide, Small and Fuller (2020) specifically highlighted how heterogeneity within Black women’s experiences may play a key role in presentation and etiology of disordered eating.
Finally, the absence of data between ages 19 and 40 did not allow us to examine how the magnitude of the association between disordered-eating symptoms in young adulthood (e.g., 20s and 30s) and adulthood may differ from the patterns we observed in adolescence. Moreover, we did not assess clinical diagnostic status to determine whether the presence of these symptoms is related to threshold-level psychopathology; however, using the EDI-3 EDRC score, we found that more than 30% of our sample appeared to be at typical or elevated clinical risk for anorexia nervosa. Fewer than 10% of the sample reached typical or elevated clinical risk for bulimia nervosa (see Table S2 in the Supplemental Material).

Conclusion

Starting in adolescence, disordered-eating cognitions and behaviors appear to exert a lifelong influence, remaining stable over the course of 28 years. Although research has focused disproportionately on these symptom presentations among white women, our results reveal that Black women, too, experience body dissatisfaction, drive for thinness, and bulimia disordered-eating symptoms. These findings affirm more recent assertions that Black women are not immune from body-image and eating disturbances, highlighting the adulthood risk associated with disordered-eating behaviors for both white and Black girls. Furthermore, in the absence of moderation by race, these results suggest that the present subscales of disordered eating manifest similarly among white and Black women, although future studies should aim to determine the degree to which racially salient disordered eating and body-image concerns are adequately captured with existing measures.

Acknowledgments

We thank the National Growth and Health Study (NGHS) research participants who gave their time and energy to this study over the course of several decades and the current NGHS research consortium for their advising on this article. Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the National Heart Lung and Blood Institute’s Growth and Health Study and does not necessarily reflect the opinions or views of those entities or individuals. Analysis scripts and deidentified data specific to the present study are available on the OSF page (https://osf.io/6dws2/). In addition, general study data for NGHS Waves I through III (adolescent data, ages 10–19) are available per National Institutes of Health guidelines at https://biolincc.nhlbi.nih.gov/studies/nghs/, along with the full study protocol, a summary of all data collected, and additional details on the study population and biospecimen collection. We report how we determined our sample size, all data exclusions, all manipulations, and all measures in the study. All research was conducted in compliance with the American Psychological Association ethical standards for the treatment of human subjects. The present secondary data analyses did not meet the definition of human subjects research per University of California Los Angeles’s Office of Human Research Protection Program and were accordingly granted an Institutional Review Board Exemption (IRB 13-000876).

ORCID iD

Footnotes

Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared that there were no conflicts of interest with respect to the authorship or the publication of this article.
Funding This work was supported by a University of California Los Angeles Graduate Research Mentorship Fellowship and the Edwin W. Pauley Fellowship to J. E. Parker, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development R01HD073568 to B. A. Laraia and E. S. Epel, and National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases R01DK128575 to A. J. Tomiyama.

Open practices

  • Open Data
  • Open Materials
  • Preregistration
This article has received the badges for Open Data, Open Materials, and Preregistration. More information about the Open Practices badges can be found at http://www.psychologicalscience.org/publications/badges.

Transparency

Action Editor: Kelsie T. Forbush
Editor: Jennifer L. Tackett
Author Contributions
Jordan E. Parker: Conceptualization; Formal analysis; Funding acquisition; Methodology; Writing–original draft; Writing–review & editing.
Jordan A. Levinson: Conceptualization; Methodology; Writing–original draft; Writing–review & editing.
Jeffrey M. Hunger: Conceptualization; Supervision; Writing–review & editing.
Craig K. Enders: Formal analysis; Methodology; Software; Supervision; Writing–original draft; Writing–review & editing.
Barbara A. Laraia: Funding acquisition; Investigation; Supervision; Writing–review & editing.
Elissa S. Epel: Funding acquisition; Investigation; Supervision; Writing–review & editing.
A. Janet Tomiyama: Conceptualization; Funding acquisition; Investigation; Methodology; Supervision; Writing–original draft; Writing–review & editing.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disordersv(5th ed.). https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
Bardone-Cone A. M., Boyd C. A. (2007). Psychometric properties of eating disorder instruments in Black and White young women: Internal consistency, temporal stability, and validity. Psychological Assessment, 19(3), 356–362. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.19.3.356
Bartholdy S., Allen K., Hodsoll J., O’Daly O. G., Campbell I. C., Banaschewski T., Bokde A. L. W., Bromberg U., Büchel C., Quinlan E. B., Conrod P. J., Desrivières S., Flor H., Frouin V., Gallinat J., Garavan H., Heinz A., Ittermann B., Martinot J.-L., . . . Schmidt U. (2017). Identifying disordered eating behaviours in adolescents: How do parent and adolescent reports differ by sex and age? European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 26(6), 691–701. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-016-0935-1
Benjamini Y., Hochberg Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society B: Methodological, 57(1), 289–300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x
Brytek-Matera A., Czepczor K. (2017). Models of eating disorders: A theoretical investigation of abnormal eating patterns and body image disturbance. Archives of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, 19(1), 16–26. https://doi.org/10.12740/APP/68422
Bucchianeri M. M., Arikian A. J., Hannan P. J., Eisenberg M. E., Neumark-Sztainer D. (2013). Body dissatisfaction from adolescence to young adulthood: Findings from a 10-year longitudinal study. Body Image, 10(1), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.09.001
Cachelin F. M., Veisel C., Barzegarnazari E., Striegel-Moore R. H. (2000). Disordered eating, acculturation, and treatment-seeking in a community sample of Hispanic, Asian, Black, and White women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24(3), 244–233. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.2000.tb00206.x
Capodilupo C. M., Kim S. (2014). Gender and race matter: The importance of considering intersections in Black women’s body image. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 61(1), 37–49. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034597
Casiero D., Frishman W. H. (2006). Cardiovascular complications of eating disorders. Cardiology in Review, 14(5), 227–231. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.crd.0000216745.96062.7c
Cassidy O., Sbrocco T., Tanofsky-Kraff M. (2015). Utilising non-traditional research designs to explore culture-specific risk factors for eating disorders in African-American adolescents. Advances in Eating Disorders, 3(1), 91–102. https://doi.org/10.1080/21662630.2014.948470
Chambers E. C., Schechter C., Tow A., Torrens L., Kohlieber R., Calderon R. (2010). Household density and obesity in young black and white adults. Ethnicity & Disease, 20(4), 366–369.
Clausen L., Rosenvinge J. H., Friborg O., Rokkedal K. (2011). Validating the Eating Disorder Inventory-3 (EDI-3): A comparison between 561 female eating disorders patients and 878 females from the general population. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 33(1), 101–110. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-010-9207-4
Cole E. R. (2020). Demarginalizing women of color in intersectionality scholarship in psychology: A Black feminist critique. Journal of Social Issues, 76(4), 1036–1044. https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12413
Combs J. L., Pearson C. M., Zapolski T. C. B., Smith G. T. (2013). Preadolescent disordered eating predicts subsequent eating dysfunction. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 38(1), 41–49. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jss094
Crow S., Eisenberg M. E., Story M., Neumark-Sztainer D. (2006). Psychosocial and behavioral correlates of dieting among overweight and non-overweight adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 38(5), 569–574. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2005.05.019
Davis D. S., Sbrocco T., Odoms-Young A., Smith D. M. (2010). Attractiveness in African American and Caucasian women: Is beauty in the eyes of the observer? Eating Behaviors, 11(1), 25–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2009.08.004
Egbert A. H., Hunt R. A., Williams K. L., Burke N. L., Mathis K. J. (2022). Reporting racial and ethnic diversity in eating disorder research over the past 20 years. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 55(4), 455–462. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.23666
Fredrickson B. L., Roberts T.-A. (1997). Objectification theory: Toward understanding women’s lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21(2), 173–206. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1997.tb00108.x
Garner D. M. (2004). Eating Disorder Inventory 3 | EDI-3. https://www.parinc.com/Products/Pkey/103
Garner D. M., Olmstead M. P., Polivy J. (1983). Development and validation of a multidimensional eating disorder inventory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 2(2), 15–34. https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-108X(198321)2:2%3C15::AID-EAT2260020203%3E3.0.CO;2-6
Gluck M. E., Geliebter A. (2002). Racial/ethnic differences in body image and eating behaviors. Eating Behaviors, 3(2), 143–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1471-0153(01)00052-6
Goode R. W., Cowell M. M., Mazzeo S. E., Cooper-Lewter C., Forte A., Olaiya O.-I., Bulik C. M. (2020). Binge eating and binge-eating disorder in Black women: A systematic review. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 53(4), 491–507. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.23217
Gordon K. H., Castro Y., Sitnikov L., Holm-Denoma J. M. (2010). Cultural body shape ideals and eating disorder symptoms among White, Latina, and Black college women. Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16(2), 135–143. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018671
Gregorich S. E. (2006). Do self-report instruments allow meaningful comparisons across diverse population groups? Testing measurement invariance using the confirmatory factor analysis framework. Medical Care, 44(11, Suppl. 3), S78–S94. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.mlr.0000245454.12228.8f
Halbeisen G., Brandt G., Paslakis G. (2022). A plea for diversity in eating disorders research. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13, Article 820043. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.820043
Hughes E. (2021). “I’m supposed to be thick”: Managing body image anxieties among Black American women. Journal of Black Studies, 52(3), 310–330. https://doi.org/10.1177/0021934720972440
Johnson J. G., Cohen P., Kasen S., Brook J. S. (2002). Eating disorders during adolescence and the risk for physical and mental disorders during early adulthood. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(6), 545–552. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.59.6.545
Kashubeck-West S., Coker A. D., Awad G. H., Stinson R. D., Bledman R., Mintz L. (2013). Do measures commonly used in body image research perform adequately with African American college women? Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 19(3), 357–368. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031905
Keel P. K., Dorer D. J., Eddy K. T., Franko D., Charatan D. L., Herzog D. B. (2003). Predictors of mortality in eating disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(2), 179–183. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.60.2.179
Kelch-Oliver K., Ancis J. R. (2011). Black women’s body image: An analysis of culture-specific influences. Women & Therapy, 34(4), 345–358. https://doi.org/10.1080/02703149.2011.592065
Kelly N. R., Mitchell K. S., Gow R. W., Trace S. E., Lydecker J. A., Bair C. E., Mazzeo S. (2012). An evaluation of the reliability and construct validity of eating disorder measures in white and black women. Psychological Assessment, 24(3), 608–617. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026457
Kempa M. L., Thomas A. J. (2000). Culturally sensitive assessment and treatment of eating disorders. Eating Disorders, 8(1), 17–30. https://doi.org/10.1080/10640260008251209
Klemchuk H. P., Hutchinson C. B., Frank R. I. (1990). Body dissatisfaction and eating-related problems on the college campus: Usefulness of the Eating Disorder Inventory with a nonclinical population. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 37(3), 297–305. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.37.3.297
Kotler L. A., Cohen P., Davies M., Pine D. S., Walsh B. T. (2001). Longitudinal relationships between childhood, adolescent, and adult eating disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(12), 1434–1440. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200112000-00014
Lovejoy M. (2001). Disturbances in the social body: Differences in body image and eating problems among African American and white women. Gender & Society, 15(2), 239–261. https://doi.org/10.1177/089124301015002005
Lowy A. S., Rodgers R. F., Franko D. L., Pluhar E., Webb J. B. (2021). Body image and internalization of appearance ideals in Black women: An update and call for culturally-sensitive research. Body Image, 39, 313–327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2021.10.005
McClelland J., Robinson L., Potterton R., Mountford V., Schmidt U. (2020). Symptom trajectories into eating disorders: A systematic review of longitudinal, nonclinical studies in children/adolescents. European Psychiatry, 63(1), Article e60. https://doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2020.55
Mikhail M. E., Klump K. L. (2021). A virtual issue highlighting eating disorders in people of black/African and Indigenous heritage. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 54(3), 459–467. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.23402
Mucherah W., Frazier A. D. (2013). How deep is skin-deep? The relationship between skin color satisfaction, estimation of body image, and self-esteem among women of African descent. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43(6), 1177–1184. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12081
The National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute Growth and Health Study & Research Group. (1992). Obesity and cardiovascular disease risk factors in black and white girls: The NHLBI Growth and Health Study. American Journal of Public Health, 82(12), 1613–1620. https://doi.org/10.2105/ajph.82.12.1613
Neumark-Sztainer D., Story M., Dixon L. B., Murray D. M. (1998). Adolescents engaging in unhealthy weight control behaviors: Are they at risk for other health-compromising behaviors? American Journal of Public Health, 88(6), 952–955. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.88.6.952
Nyman-Carlsson E., Engström I., Norring C., Nevonen L. (2015). Eating Disorder Inventory-3, validation in Swedish patients with eating disorders, psychiatric outpatients and a normal control sample. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 69(2), 142–151. https://doi.org/10.3109/08039488.2014.949305
Ordaz D. L., Schaefer L. M., Choquette E., Schueler J., Wallace L., Thompson J. K. (2018). Thinness pressures in ethnically diverse college women in the United States. Body Image, 24, 1–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.11.004
Parker J. E., Enders C. K., Mujahid M. S., Laraia B. A., Epel E. S., Tomiyama A. J. (2022). Prospective relationships between skin color satisfaction, body satisfaction, and binge eating in Black girls. Body Image, 41, 342–353. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2022.04.004
Perez M., Joiner T. E. (2003). Body image dissatisfaction and disordered eating in black and white women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 33(3), 342–350. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.10148
Quick V. M., Byrd-Bredbenner C. (2014). Disordered eating, socio-cultural media influencers, body image, and psychological factors among a racially/ethnically diverse population of college women. Eating Behaviors, 15(1), 37–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.10.005
Rancourt D., Choquette E. M., Ahlich E., Lang B. M., Verzijl C. L., Palermo M., Belzak W. C. M. (2022). Invariance of the Eating Disorder Inventory-Drive for Thinness subscale across university and community samples. Psychological Assessment, 34(4), 341–352. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0001095
Roberts S. O., Bareket-Shavit C., Dollins F. A., Goldie P. D., Mortenson E. (2020). Racial inequality in psychological research: Trends of the past and recommendations for the future. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(6), 1295–1309. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620927709
Rodgers R. F., Berry R., Franko D. L. (2018). Eating disorders in ethnic minorities: An update. Current Psychiatry Reports, 20(10), 90. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-018-0938-3
Rosen J. C., Silberg N. T., Gross J. (1988). Eating Attitudes Test and Eating Disorders Inventory: Norms for adolescent girls and boys. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(2), 305–308. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-006x.56.2.305
Roth W. D. (2016). The multiple dimensions of race. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 39(8), 1310–1338. https://doi.org/10.1080/01419870.2016.1140793
Rothstein L. A., Sbrocco T., Carter M. M. (2017). Factor analysis of EDI-3 eating disorder risk subscales among African American women. Journal of Black Psychology, 43(8), 767–777. https://doi.org/10.1177/0095798417708506
Rucker C. E., Cash T. F. (1992). Body images, body-size perceptions, and eating behaviors among African-American and white college women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 12(3), 291–299. https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-108X(199211)12:3%3C291::AID-EAT2260120309%3E3.0.CO;2-A
Santomauro D. F., Melen S., Mitchison D., Vos T., Whiteford H., Ferrari A. J. (2021). The hidden burden of eating disorders: An extension of estimates from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. The Lancet Psychiatry, 8(4), 320–328. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(21)00040-7
Schaefer L. M., Burke N. L., Calogero R. M., Menzel J. E., Krawczyk R., Thompson J. K. (2018). Self-objectification, body shame, and disordered eating: Testing a core mediational model of objectification theory among White, Black, and Hispanic women. Body Image, 24, 5–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.10.005
Small C., Fuller M. (Eds.). (2020). Treating Black women with eating disorders: A clinician’s guide. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003011743
Sonneville K. R., Lipson S. K. (2018). Disparities in eating disorder diagnosis and treatment according to weight status, race/ethnicity, socioeconomic background, and sex among college students. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 51(6), 518–526. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22846
Stice E., Marti C. N., Durant S. (2011). Risk factors for onset of eating disorders: Evidence of multiple risk pathways from an 8-year prospective study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49(10), 622–627. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.06.009
Stice E., Van Ryzin M. J. (2019). A prospective test of the temporal sequencing of risk factor emergence in the dual pathway model of eating disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 128(2), 119–128. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000400
Striegel-Moore R. H., Dohm F. A., Kraemer H. C., Taylor C. B., Daniels S., Crawford P. B., Schreiber G. B. (2003). Eating disorders in white and Black women. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(7), 1326–1331. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.7.1326
Striegel-Moore R. H., Schreiber G. B., Lo A., Crawford P., Obarzanek E., Rodin J. (2000). Eating disorder symptoms in a cohort of 11 to 16-year-old black and white girls: The NHLBI growth and health study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 27(1), 49–66. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(200001)27:1%3C49::AID-EAT6%3E3.0.CO;2-E
Striegel-Moore R. H., Wilfley D. E., Pike K. M., Dohm F. A., Fairburn C. G. (2000). Recurrent binge eating in black American women. Archives of Family Medicine, 9(1), 83–87. https://doi.org/10.1001/archfami.9.1.83
Taylor J. Y., Caldwell C. H., Baser R. E., Matusko N., Faison N., Jackson J. S. (2013). Classification and correlates of eating disorders among Blacks: Findings from the national survey of American life. Journal of Health Care for the Poor and Underserved, 24(1), 289–310. https://doi.org/10.1353/hpu.2013.0027
Voelker D. K., Reel J. J., Greenleaf C. (2015). Weight status and body image perceptions in adolescents: Current perspectives. Adolescent Health, Medicine and Therapeutics, 6, 149–158. https://doi.org/10.2147/AHMT.S68344
Watson L. B., Lewis J. A., Moody A. T. (2019). A sociocultural examination of body image among Black women. Body Image, 31, 280–287. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.03.008
Watson L. B., Robinson D., Dispenza F., Nazari N. (2012). African American women’s sexual objectification experiences: A qualitative study. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 36(4), 458–475. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684312454724
White M. A., Grilo C. M. (2005). Ethnic differences in the prediction of eating and body image disturbances among female adolescent psychiatric inpatients. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 38(1), 78–84. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20142
Wildes J. E., Emery R. E., Simons A. D. (2001). The roles of ethnicity and culture in the development of eating disturbance and body dissatisfaction: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 21(4), 521–551. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(99)00071-9
Wilfley D. E., Schreiber G. B., Pike K. M., Striegel-Moore R. H., Wright D. J., Rodin J. (1996). Eating disturbance and body image: A comparison of a community sample of adult black and white women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 20(4), 377–387. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199612)20:4%3C377::AID-EAT5%3E3.0.CO;2-K
Wilfred S. A., Lundgren J. D. (2021). The Double Consciousness Body Image Scale: A body image assessment centering the experiences of Black women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 54(10), 1771–1781. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.23581
Williamson L. (1998). Eating disorders and the cultural forces behind the drive for thinness: Are African American women really protected? Social Work in Health Care, 28(1), 61–73. https://doi.org/10.1300/J010v28n01_04

Supplementary Material

Please find the following supplemental material available below.

For Open Access articles published under a Creative Commons License, all supplemental material carries the same license as the article it is associated with.

For non-Open Access articles published, all supplemental material carries a non-exclusive license, and permission requests for re-use of supplemental material or any part of supplemental material shall be sent directly to the copyright owner as specified in the copyright notice associated with the article.

Cite article

Cite article

Cite article

OR

Download to reference manager

If you have citation software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice

Share options

Share

Share this article

Share with email
Email Article Link
Share on social media

Share access to this article

Sharing links are not relevant where the article is open access and not available if you do not have a subscription.

For more information view the Sage Journals article sharing page.

Information, rights and permissions

Information

Published In

Article first published online: February 22, 2023
Issue published: September 2023

Keywords

  1. body dissatisfaction
  2. bulimia
  3. Black women
  4. disordered eating
  5. longitudinal
  6. open data
  7. open materials
  8. preregistered

Rights and permissions

© The Author(s) 2023.
Creative Commons License (CC BY-NC 4.0)
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits non-commercial use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).
Request permissions for this article.

Open practices

  • Open Data
  • Open Materials
  • Preregistration

Authors

Affiliations

Jordan E. Parker
Department of Psychology, University of California Los Angeles
Jordan A. Levinson
Department of Psychology, University of California Los Angeles
Jeffrey M. Hunger
Department of Psychology, Miami University
Craig K. Enders
Department of Psychology, University of California Los Angeles
Barbara A. Laraia
School of Public Health, Division of Public Health Nutrition, University of California Berkeley
Elissa S. Epel
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, UCSF Weill Institute for Neurosciences, & Center for Health and Community, University of California San Francisco
A. Janet Tomiyama
Department of Psychology, University of California Los Angeles

Notes

A. Janet Tomiyama, Department of Psychology, University of California Los Angeles Email: [email protected]

Metrics and citations

Metrics

Journals metrics

This article was published in Clinical Psychological Science.

View All Journal Metrics

Article usage*

Total views and downloads: 1946

*Article usage tracking started in December 2016


Altmetric

See the impact this article is making through the number of times it’s been read, and the Altmetric Score.
Learn more about the Altmetric Scores



Articles citing this one

Receive email alerts when this article is cited

Web of Science: 0 view articles Opens in new tab

Crossref: 0

There are no citing articles to show.

Figures and tables

Figures & Media

Tables

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Access options

If you have access to journal content via a personal subscription, university, library, employer or society, select from the options below:

APS members can access this journal content using society membership credentials.

APS members can access this journal content using society membership credentials.


Alternatively, view purchase options below:

Purchase 24 hour online access to view and download content.

Access journal content via a DeepDyve subscription or find out more about this option.