Interpersonal Autonomic Physiology: A Systematic Review of the Literature
Abstract
Get full access to this article
View all access and purchase options for this article.
Appendices
Reference | Population | Term for physiological synchrony | Statistic of physiological synchrony | Methodology (idiographic or nomothetic) | Null hypothesis tested | Physiological measures |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bachrach, Fontbonne, Joufflineau, and Ulloa (2015) | Other: Dancers and audience | Entrainment | Regression | Both | No | RR |
Baker et al. (2015) | Mother–child | Biobehavioral synchrony | Correlation | Nomothetic | No | EDA |
Chanel, Kivikangas, and Ravaja (2012) | Teammates | Compliance | Correlation; Weighted coherence | Nomothetic | No | HF-HRV; HF-Resp-Amp; IBI; Resp-Amp; SC; VLF-HRV |
Chatel-Goldman, Congedo, Jutten, and Schwartz (2014) | Couples | Physiological coupling | Cross-correlations | Nomothetic | Yes | RVT; PRV; SC |
Codrons, Bernardi, Vandoni, and Bernardi (2014) | Other: Student group | Entrainment | Generalized Partial directed coherence | Nomothetic | Yes | RR; HP |
Coleman, Greenblatt, and Solomon (1956) | Therapist–client | Physiological relationship | Correlation | Idiographic | No | HR |
Creaven, Skowron, Hughes, Howard, and Loken (2014) | Mother–child | Concordance | Multilevel model | Nomothetic | No | HR; RSA |
Di Mascio, Boyd, Greenblatt, and Solomon (1955) | Therapist–client | Sociophysiology | Correlation | Idiographic | No | PR |
Ebisch et al. (2012) | Mother–child | Synchrony | Correlation | Nomothetic | No | Temp |
Elkins et al. (2009) | Teammates | Compliance | Signal matching; Instantaneous derivative matching; Directional agreement; Correlation | Nomothetic | No | IBI; RSA |
Feldman, Magori-Cohen, Galili, Singer, and Louzoun (2011) | Mother–child | Biological synchrony | ARIMA model with cross-correlation function | Nomothetic | Yes | IBI |
Ferrer and Helm (2013) | Couples | Covariation | Differential equation models | Both | Yes | HR; Resp |
Field, Healy, and LeBlanc (1989) | Mother–child | Synchrony | Coherence and cross-coherence | Nomothetic | No | HR |
Field et al. (1992) | Other: Friends–acquaintances | Coherence | Coherence | Nomothetic | No | HR |
Gates, Gatzke-Kopp, Sandsten, and Blandon (2015) | Couples | Linkage | Cross-correlation | Nomothetic | No | RSA |
Ghafar-Tabrizi (2008) | Mother–child | Linkage | Bivariate time-series analysis | Both | No | HR; FPA |
Goldstein, Field, and Healy (1989) | Other: Friends–acquaintances | Concordance | Repeated-measures ANOVA | Nomothetic | No | HR |
Guastello, Pincus, and Gunderson (2006) | Other: Classmates | Linkage | Linear regression; Nonlinear regression | Both | No | SC |
Ham and Tronick (2009) | Mother–child | Concordance | Windowed correlation of slope | Nomothetic | No | SC |
Helm, Sbarra, and Ferrer (2012) | Couples | Covariation | Coupled oscillator models | Nomothetic | Yes | HR; Resp |
Helm, Sbarra, and Ferrer (2014) | Couples | Synchrony | Cross-lagged panel models | Both | No | RSA |
Henning, Armstead, and Ferris (2009) | Teammates | Compliance | Cross-correlation | Nomothetic | No | HRV |
Henning, Boucsein, and Gil (2001) | Teammates | Compliance | Cross-correlation; Weighted cross-coherence | Nomothetic | No | HRV; RR; SC |
Henning and Korbelak (2005) | Teammates | Compliance | Cross-correlation | Nomothetic | No | IBI |
Hill-Soderlund et al. (2008) | Mother–child | Physiological Attunement | General linear model with repeated-measures and time-varying covariates | Nomothetic | No | RSA |
Järvelä, Kivikangas, Kätsyri, and Ravaja (2013) | Teammates | Linkage | Cross-correlation; Weighted cross-coherence | Nomothetic | No | IBI; HF-HRV; SCR; SCL |
Kaplan, Burch, Bloom, and Edelberg (1963) | Other: Classmates | Covariation | Correlation | Nomothetic | No | SCR |
Konvalinka et al. (2011) | Other: Familial vs. non-familial | Synchronized arousal | Cross-recurrence quantification analysis | Both | No | HR |
Kraus and Mendes (2014) | Other: Mock businessmen | Affect contagion | Lagged correlation | Nomothetic | No | PEP |
Levenson and Gottman (1983) | Couples | Linkage | Bivariate time-series analysis | Nomothetic | No | HR; PTT; SCL; ACTa; Physiological index (HR; PTT; SCL; ACTa) |
Liu, Zhou, Palumbo, and Wang (in press) | Couples | Linkage | Dynamic correlation | Nomothetic | Yes | SC |
Lunkenheimer et al. (2015) | Mother–child | Synchrony | Multilevel coupled autoregressive model | Both | No | RSA |
Manini et al. (2013) | Mother–child | Attunement | Correlations; Cross-correlations | Both | No | Temp |
Marci (2006) | Other: Audience members | Emotional engagement | Proprietary analysis not divulged | Unspecified | No | Physiological index (SC; HR; RR; ACTa) |
Marci, Ham, Moran, and Orr (2007) | Therapist–client | Concordance | Windowed correlation of slope | Nomothetic | Yes | SCL |
Marci and Orr (2006) | Therapist–client | Concordance | Windowed correlation of slope | Nomothetic | No | SCL |
McAssey, Helm, Hsieh, Sbarra, and Ferrer (2013) | Couples | Synchrony | Structural heteroscedastic measurement-error model; Empirical mode decomposition | Both | Yes | HR; RR; TI |
McFarland (2001) | Other: Friends | Synchrony | Cross-correlation | Nomothetic | Yes | RR |
Messina et al. (2013) | Therapist–client | Concordance | Windowed correlation of slope | Both | No | SCL |
Mitkidis, McGraw, Roepstorff, and Wallot (2015) | Other: randomly paired students | Synchrony | Cross-recurrence quantification analysis | Both | No | HR |
Montague, Xu, and Chiou (2014) | Teams | Compliance | Signal matching; Instantaneous derivative matching; Directional agreement; Cross-correlation; Weighted coherence | Nomothetic | Yes | IBI; HF-HRV; LF-HRV; SC |
Moore (2009) | Mother–child | None | Correlation | Nomothetic | No | RSA |
Moore et al. (2009) | Mother–child | Synchrony | Correlation | Nomothetic | No | HP; RSA |
Morgan, Gunes, and Bryan-Kinns (2015) | Other: Musicians | Physiological linkage | Percentage of matched slope | Nomothetic | No | HR |
Muller and Lindenberger (2011) | Other: Choir members | Synchrony | Wavelet coherence; Granger causality (multivariate autoregressive model) | Both | No | HRV; Resp |
Noy, Levit-Binun, and Golland (2015) | Other: Experts at joint improvisation | Synchrony | Correlation | Nomothetic | No | HR |
Quer, Daftari, and Rao (in press) | Other: Groups meditating | Entrainment | Wavelet coherence | Idiographic | No | HF-HRV; LF-HRV; VLF-HR; RR |
Reed, Randall, Post, and Butler (2013) | Couples | Linkage | Longitudinal multilevel dyadic model | Nomothetic | Yes | BP; IBI; SC |
Robinson, Herman, and Kaplan (1982) | Therapist–client | Concordance | Correlation; Discrete categorizations of SCR | Nomothetic | No | SCR; FST |
Shearn, Spellman, Straley, Meirick, and Stryker (1999) | Other: Friends–strangers | Shared physiology | Correlations | Nomothetic | No | SC; Blush |
Silver and Parente (2004) | Other: Strangers | Shared physiological reaction | Correlation | Nomothetic | No | SC |
Strang, Funke, Russell, Dukes, and Middendorf (2014) | Teammates | Physio-behavioral coupling | cross-correlation, cross-recurrence quantification analysis, and cross-fuzzy entropy | Both | Yes | IBI |
Stratford, Lal, and Meara (2009) | Therapist–client | Concordance | Windowed correlation of slope | Nomothetic | No | SC |
Stratford, Lal, and Meara (2012) | Therapist–client | Concordance | Windowed correlation of slope | Nomothetic | No | SC |
Suveg, Shaffer, and Davis (2016) | Mother–child | Synchrony | Cross-correlation | Nomothetic | No | IBI |
Thomsen and Gilbert (1998) | Couples | Linkage | Bivariate time-series analysis | Nomothetic | No | HR; SC |
Van Puyvelde et al. (2015) | Mother–child | Synchrony | Difference score | Nomothetic | No | RSA |
Vickhoff et al. (2013) | Other: Choir singers | Inter-individual synchronization | Cross-coherence | Both | No | HRV |
Walker, Muth, Switzer, and Rosopa (2013) | Teammates | Compliance | Regressions and correlations | Nomothetic | No | Physiological index (RSA; PEP; LVET) |
Waters, West, and Mendes (2014) | Mother–child | Covariation | Growth curve model | Nomothetic | No | HR; VC |
Woltering, Lishak, Elliott, Ferraro, and Granic (2015) | Mother–child | Synchrony | Structural heteroscedastic measurement-error model | Nomothetic | No | HR |
Source | Measure | Context | Finding | Results |
---|---|---|---|---|
Coleman, Greenblatt, and Solomon (1956) | HR | General | S | Significant correlations found between client and therapist HR. |
Creaven, Skowron, Hughes, Howard, and Loken (2014) | HR | General | S | Positive concurrent PS between mother and child HR was observed during the resting condition. |
Between | S | Magnitudes of PS in HR significantly greater in the maltreating group. When a child maltreating mother displayed lower relative HR, her child displayed lower HR. Conversely, higher maternal HR was associated with higher child HR in non-maltreating dyads. | ||
Moderator | S | Mother–child PS in HR was moderated by mothers’ average HR, such that mothers with greater HR elevations had lower PS, and those with lower average HR had greater PS. | ||
Ferrer and Helm (2013) | HR | Within | S | Change across tasks (baseline, gazing, and imitation) was significant for some participants, although on average it was not. |
Typology | S | Negative PS was found, such that when parameters for one partner decreased, the other partner’s increased. Positive PS in HR was found in more than half of the dyads. | ||
PsychoSoc | Females’ PS in HR during the imitation task was related to daily affect, suggesting that females’ physiology adjusted to their male partners’ in this task similarly to adjustments in response to their partners’ daily affect. | |||
Field et al. (1992) | HR | Between | NS | No statistically significant difference in PS between friend and acquaintance dyads. |
Field, Healy, and LeBlanc (1989) | HR | Between | NS | No significant differences in PS magnitudes across depressed and non-depressed dyads. |
Ghafar-Tabrizi (2008) | HR | Between | S | In the high-conflict group when daughters led the conversation, daughter’s HR predicted mother’s HR significantly better than when mothers led the conversation. |
Within | NS | Equivalent levels of PS demonstrated across varied conversation topics. | ||
S | When daughters led the conversation, their HR predicted the mother’s HR better than mother’s HR predicted daughter’s, and vice versa. | |||
PsychoSoc | S | Levels of felt and preferred arousal were associated with the magnitude of PS during dyadic interaction. | ||
Goldstein, Field, and Healy (1989) | HR | Between | S | Greater PS for friends vs. acquaintance dyads. During pre-play baseline, friends showed significantly greater PS than acquaintances. |
Helm, Sbarra, and Ferrer (2012) | NS | No significant differences in PS between friend and acquaintance dyads during play period. | ||
HR | Within | S | Different patterns of PS were found in couples across baseline, gazing, and imitation tasks. | |
Typology | S | Patterns suggesting both positive and negative PS present in couples during baseline and gazing tasks, but only negative PS was observed during imitation task. | ||
PsychoSoc | S | Higher anxiety and avoidance were associated with greater magnitudes of PS in couples during the imitation task, but not during the baseline or gazing tasks. | ||
NS | Neither relationship length or satisfaction was related to couples’ PS in HR. | |||
Konvalinka et al. (2011) | HR | Between | S | PS was found between fire walkers and familial spectators during ritual fire walking, but not between fire walkers and non-familial spectators. |
Levenson and Gottman (1983) | HR | Within | NS | PS in HR was not detected when couples were discussing neutral or conflict topics. |
PsychoSoc | NS | PS in HR was not predictive of marital satisfaction, affect, or affect reciprocity. | ||
McAssey, Helm, Hsieh, Sbarra, and Ferrer (2013) | HR | Within | S | No PS during baseline, and significant PS for some couples during gazing and imitation task. No significant results were seen in randomly paired participant data. |
Mitkidis, McGraw, Roepstorff, and Wallot (2015) | HR | Between | S | PS was significantly greater in student dyad groups that played an economic trust game (public goods game) following a joint cooperative task, compared with groups who did not play the trust game. |
Within | S | In the group that played the public goods game, PS was positively associated with expectations of returns but not of investments (i.e., mock money they thought they would get back, vs. mock money they would invest). The authors concluded that PS could be viewed as a proxy measure of trust. | ||
Morgan, Gunes, and Bryan-Kinns (2015) | HR | General | NS | No PS was detected between drummers’ HR. |
Noy, Levit-Binun, and Golland (2015) | HR | General | S | Significant PS in HR was found between dyads mirroring each other’s movements. PS was significantly, positively correlated with behavioral synchrony, and heart rate. |
PsychoSoc | S | Significant positive correlation found between PS in HR and dyads’ self-reported experiences of togetherness. | ||
Stratford, Lal, and Meara (2009) | HR | Within | S | Patterns of EEG during high PS differed across therapy sessions. |
Stratford, Lal, and Meara (2012) | HR | Within | S | Patterns of EEG during high PS differed across therapy sessions. |
Thomsen and Gilbert (1998) | HR | General | S | PS was detected in couples’ HR when discussing a conflict topic, but results varied across dyads |
Woltering, Lishak, Elliott, Ferraro, and Granic (2015) | HR | Within | S | PS in HR observed between mother–child dyads during positive and negative discussions. More than twice the magnitude of PS observed during the last vs. first topic, suggesting increased attempts at repair. |
NS | PS in HR between mother–child dyads associated with behavioral synchrony during negative, but not positive, discussion. | |||
Group | NS | No difference in mother–child PS in HR between children with typical and atypical self-regulation during discussions. | ||
PsychoSoc | S | Increased PS significantly predicted the degree of “repair” in mother–child relationship following a negative discussion, for both children with and without self-regulation issues. | ||
Creaven, Skowron, Hughes, Howard, and Loken (2014) | HR/RSA | General | S | PS was observed in mother HR and child RSA over time. |
Moderator | S | Higher resting maternal HR was associated with significantly lower PS in mother HR and child RSA. | ||
Waters, West, and Mendes (2014) | HR/VC | General | S | Significant PS was found between infant HR and mother VC. |
Within | S | PS between infant HR and mother VC was found in the negative and positive stress conditions, but not the neutral condition. PS increased over time in negative stress condition, but not positive or neutral conditions. | ||
Di Mascio, Boyd, Greenblatt, and Solomon (1955) | PR | General | S | PS of pulse rate fluctuations found between patient and doctor during a psychiatric interview. |
Typology | S | Pulse rates observed to vary together or inversely, labeled concordance and discordance (i.e., positive and negative PS). | ||
Chatel-Goldman, Congedo, Jutten, and Schwartz (2014) | PRV | General | NA | Significance of PS between couples’ pulse rate variability was not tested. |
Within | NS | No significant differences in PS of pulse rate variability when couples could or could not touch each other. | ||
PsychoSoc | NS | No significant correlations between empathy scores and PS in pulse rate variability found. | ||
Levenson and Gottman (1983) | PTT | Within | NS | PS in PTT was not detected when couples were discussing neutral or conflict topics. |
PsychoSoc | NS | PS in PTT was not predictive of marital satisfaction, affect, or affect reciprocity. | ||
Ghafar-Tabrizi (2008) | FPA | Between | S | PS was stronger during conflict than pleasant conversation for the high-conflict group only. |
PsychoSoc | S | Levels of preferred, but not felt, arousal were associated with the strength of PS in finger pulse amplitude during dyadic interaction. | ||
Reed, Randall, Post, and Butler (2013) | BP | PsychoSoc | S | At low levels of negative influence, PS in BP was significantly negative, but at high levels of negative influence, BP was unsynchronized. When demand behaviors were not present, PS in BP was significantly negative, while during demand behaviors, PS was positive. When withdraw behaviors were not present, PS in BP was negative. When withdraw behaviors were present, PS was significantly positive. |
McAssey, Helm, Hsieh, Sbarra, and Ferrer (2013) | TI | Within | S | Significant increase in PS in thoracic impedance from baseline to the gazing and in-sync tasks for some couples. |
Chanel, Kivikangas, and Ravaja (2012) | IBI | General | S | Teammates’ PS in IBI positive and significantly different from zero. |
Within | S | Teammates’ PS in IBI higher during competitive vs. cooperative play. | ||
PsychoSoc | NS | No self-report gaming experience constructs were significantly related to PS in IBI. | ||
Elkins et al. (2009) | IBI | Between | NS | Trend-level differences in mean PS in IBI found between teams with high and low performance, but did not reach significance. |
Feldman, Magori-Cohen, Galili, Singer, and Louzoun (2011) | IBI | General | S | Statistically significant levels of PS in IBI were found during face-to-face interactions between mothers and infants. |
Moderator | S | Time periods involving vocal synchrony, affect synchrony, or the co-occurrence of vocal and affect synchrony between mothers and infants were significantly related to increased PS in IBI. | ||
Henning and Korbelak (2005) | IBI | General | S | There was a small significant effect indicating that PS in IBI predicted some measures of team performance. |
Järvelä, Kivikangas, Kätsyri, and Ravaja (2013) | IBI | General | S | PS present among team members. |
Within | S | PS highest during competitive conditions without a computerized opponent. | ||
Moderator | S | Previous experience with the game minimized conditional differences in PS. | ||
PsychoSoc | S | Increased empathy and understanding between players associated with increased PS in IBI. | ||
Montague, Xu, and Chiou (2014) | IBI | Within | S | PS in teammates’ IBI was higher during team trials than during baseline. |
NS | A number of metrics of team performance and experience were not correlated with PS in IBI. | |||
Reed, Randall, Post, and Butler (2013) | IBI | General | NS | PS in IBI was not observed during couples’ conversations. |
PsychoSoc | NS | PS in IBI was not observed and not significantly different across contexts of couples’ conversations. | ||
Strang, Funke, Russell, Dukes, and Middendorf (2014) | IBI | General | NS | PS in IBI was not significantly greater than PS from randomly paired dyads. |
Suveg, Shaffer, and Davis (2016) | IBI | General | S | PS was detected between mothers’ and their children during a joint task. |
NS | PS was not detected between mothers and their children during a silent baseline. | |||
Moderator | S | Family risk moderated the relationship between physiological and behavioral synchrony between mothers and their children. Higher risk families showed a negative association between behavioral and physiological synchrony. Family risk moderated the relation between PS and child self-regulation. High-risk children had a negative relation between self-regulation and PS. | ||
NS | There were no moderating effects between behavioral and physiological synchrony or child-self-regulation in low-risk families. | |||
Within | S | PS in IBI was equal during high and low arousal, in both high- and low-risk dyads. | ||
Between | S | PS was negatively correlated with behavioral synchrony in high-risk families. | ||
Henning, Armstead, and Ferris (2009) | HRV | Within | S | Teammates’ PS in HRV negatively associated with ratings for team productivity. |
NS | Teammates’ PS in HRV not significantly associated with ratings of individual participation, workload, or effort for decisions. | |||
PsychoSoc | S | Teammates’ PS in HRV negatively associated with ratings for quality of communication, and ability to work together. | ||
Henning, Boucsein, and Gil (2001) | HRV | Within | S | PS in HRV a significant predictor of some measures of team performance. |
NS | Teammates’ PS in HRV not significantly correlated with team behavioral coordination. | |||
Muller and Lindenberger (2011) | HRV | Within | S | PS in HRV was significantly stronger during singing than during rest. Directed positive PS where physiological changes in the conductor were followed by choir members. |
Vickhoff et al. (2013) | HRV | Within | S | PS in choir members’ HRV was detected during the hymn and mantra singing. |
NS | PS in choir members’ HRV was not detected during silent baselines, or when members were humming. | |||
Chanel, Kivikangas, and Ravaja (2012) | HF-HRV | Within | NS | Teammates’ PS in HF-HRV at the home and at the lab not significantly different. |
S | Teammates’ PS in HF-HRV significantly higher for cooperative play as compared with competitive play. | |||
PsychoSoc | S | Teammates’ PS in HF-HRV was positively associated with social negative feelings. | ||
Järvelä, Kivikangas, Kätsyri, and Ravaja (2013) | HF-HRV | General | S | PS in HF-HRV was present among team members. |
PsychoSoc | S | PS in HF-HRV was positively associated with self-report ratings of perceived comprehension and negatively with behavioral involvement. | ||
Montague, Xu, and Chiou (2014) | HF-HRV | Within | S | PS in teammates’ HF-HRV during difficult team virtual tasks and when technology was unreliable was significantly higher than in other conditions. Also, PS in HF-HRV was negatively correlated with teams’ trust in the technology. |
NS | A number of measures of team performance were not significantly correlated with teams’ PS in HF-HRV. | |||
Quer, Daftari, and Rao (in press) | HF-HRV | Within | S | Group-level PS in HF-HRV was detected in groups meditating, chanting, and performing breathing exercises. |
Montague, Xu, and Chiou (2014) | LF-HRV | Within | NS | A number of measures of team performance and experience were not significantly correlated with teams’ PS in LF-HRV. |
Quer, Daftari, and Rao (in press) | LF-HRV | Within | S | Group-level PS in LF-HRV was detected in groups meditating, chanting, and performing breathing exercises. |
Chanel, Kivikangas, and Ravaja (2012) | VLF-HRV | Within | S | Teammates’ PS in VLF-HRV significantly higher at home than in the lab. |
Quer, Daftari, and Rao (in press) | VLF-HRV | Within | S | Group-level PS in VLF-HRV was detected in groups meditating, chanting, and performing breathing exercises. |
Creaven, Skowron, Hughes, Howard, and Loken (2014) | RSA | General | NS | No significant PS in mother and child RSA. |
Moderator | NS | Maltreatment status not a significant moderator of mother–child PS in RSA. | ||
Elkins et al. (2009) | RSA | Between | S | Teammates in high- and low-performance groups had significantly different levels of PS in RSA. |
Gates, Gatzke-Kopp, Sandsten, and Blandon (2015) | RSA | PsychoSoc | S | PS in RSA was significantly and positively correlated with self-reported marital conflict. |
Helm, Sbarra, and Ferrer (2014) | RSA | General | S | Results indicated that PS in RSA was significantly different from zero. |
Within | S | PS in RSA significantly increased from the baseline to the conversation tasks, and that high RSA in one partner led to higher RSA in the other partner. | ||
NS | Couples’ PS in RSA did not significantly differ by conversation type. | |||
Typology | S | Results indicated that PS followed a morphostatic pattern (i.e., bidirectional interdependence around a stable arousal level). | ||
PsychoSoc | S | PS was significantly stronger in couples with higher quality relationships. | ||
Hill-Soderlund et al. (2008) | RSA | General | NS | No significant findings of PS in RSA between mothers and infants during the strange situation paradigm. |
PsychoSoc | NS | No significant findings of PS in RSA between mothers and infants during the strange situation paradigm with attachment status as an interaction term. | ||
Lunkenheimer et al. (2015) | RSA | General | S | Models of PS explained over 30% of variance in mothers’ and children’s RSA across lab-based tasks. |
Moderator | S | PS between mother and child RSA was negative when children’s problem behaviors were high, but positive when low. | ||
Moore (2009) | RSA | General | NS | Correlations between mothers’ and their infants’ RSA change scores during the still-face paradigm were not statistically significant. |
Moore et al. (2009) | RSA | General | NS | PS in RSA was not detected between mother–infant dyads during a quiet, still 2-min baseline. |
Walker, Muth, Switzer, and Rosopa (2013) | RSA | Within | NS | PS in teammates’ RSA not a significant predictor of team errors. |
Codrons, Bernardi, Vandoni, and Bernardi (2014) | HP | Within | NS | PS in HP was not observed between groups of 10 people sitting still and quiet, or swinging their arms while in silence, listening to music, or listening to a metronome. |
Moore et al. (2009) | HP | General | S | A moderate, significant correlation between mother–infant’s HP was detected during a quiet, still 2-min baseline. |
Kraus and Mendes (2014) | PEP | Between | S | Results indicated low-status partners’ PEP was positively synchronized with high-status partners’ PEP at a 30-s lag during mock negotiations, suggesting high-status partners led the interaction. |
Van Puyvelde et al. (2015) | RSA | Within | S | Mother–infant RSA synchronized during different maternal breathing paces until infants were 2 months. |
NS | Mother–infant RSA did not synchronize during different maternal breathing paces when infants were 3 months. | |||
Walker, Muth, Switzer, and Rosopa (2013) | PEP | Within | NS | PS in teammates’ PEP not a significant predictor of team errors. |
Walker, Muth, Switzer, and Rosopa (2013) | LVET | Within | NS | PS in teammates’ LVET not a significant predictor of team errors. |
Source | Measure | Context | Finding | Results |
---|---|---|---|---|
Baker et al. (2015) | SC | Moderator | S | Strength of PS in SC was negatively associated with autism severity. More severe diagnosis was associated with lower PS. |
Chanel, Kivikangas, and Ravaja (2012) | SC | General | S | PS in SC significantly different from zero. |
PsychoSoc | S | Positive affect during gaming experience positively associated with PS in SC. | ||
NS | Other psychosocial results non-significant. | |||
Chatel-Goldman, Congedo, Jutten, and Schwartz (2014) | SC | General | S | PS in SC of couples beyond what was accounted for in data from random dyads. |
Within | S | Touch significantly increased couples’ PS in SC. | ||
PsychoSoc | NS | No main effects of empathy state on PS in SC. | ||
S | Negative correlation between “splitting emotions” and PS, as well as empathy and PS with touch, indicating touch had a stronger influence on PS between partners who were less empathic. | |||
Guastello, Pincus, and Gunderson (2006) | SC | General | S | Significant levels of PS in SC between dyads during all conversation types. |
Between | S | No statistically significant difference in PS between groups having different conversations. | ||
Ham and Tronick (2009) | SC | Within | NS | Positive PS in SC between mothers and infants approached significance during the still-face paradigm when infants displayed negative behaviors. PS in SC not significantly related to engagement behaviors between mother and infant during normal interaction. |
S | When mothers engaged in soothing of infants during reengagement, greater positive PS occurred in relation to behavioral synchrony. | |||
Henning, Boucsein, and Gil (2001) | SC | Within | S | PS in SC was a significant predictor for some measures of team performance. |
NS | PS in SC not predictive of team coordination. | |||
Liu, Zhou, Palumbo, and Wang (in press) | SC | Within | S | Significant PS was detected in couples’ SC when participants were quietly seated face-to-face. PS during this condition was significantly greater than when couples were seated back-to-back. |
NS | PS was not detected in couples’ SC when participants were quietly seated back-to-back. | |||
Reed, Randall, Post, and Butler (2013) | SC | General | NS | PS in SC not observed during couples’ conversations. |
PsychoSoc | NS | PS in SC not observed and not significantly different across contexts of couples’ conversations. | ||
Shearn, Spellman, Straley, Meirick, and Stryker (1999) | SC | Between | S | Significant PS in SC between performers and friends, but not between friends and strangers or strangers and performers. |
Silver and Parente (2004) | SC | General | S | Significant PS in SC between pairs of strangers conversing for the first time. |
Stratford, Lal, and Meara (2009) | SC | Within | S | PS in SC increased from Therapy Sessions 1 to 4. Highest PS in SC between therapists and clients recorded during Session 4. Patterns of EEG accompanying peak PS in SC differed across therapy sessions. |
Stratford, Lal, and Meara (2012) | SC | Within | S | During peak PS in SC between therapists and clients, frontal site showed significantly lower EEG beta activity during Therapy Session 6 compared with 3. |
Thomsen and Gilbert (1998) | SC | General | S | PS was detected in couples’ SC when discussing a conflict topic, but results were varied across dyads. Husbands’ SC was a significantly better predictor of wives’ SC than vice versa. |
Järvelä, Kivikangas, Kätsyri, and Ravaja (2013) | SCL | General | S | PS in SCL significantly higher than zero among team members. |
Within | NS | PS in SCL not significantly different in cooperative/competitive conditions or including computer/non-computer players. | ||
PsychoSoc | NS | PS in SCL not significantly related to teammates’ emotional or behavioral self-report items. | ||
Levenson and Gottman (1983) | SCL | Within | NS | PS in SCL not detected when couples discussing neutral or conflict topics. |
PsychoSoc | NS | PS in SCL not predictive of marital satisfaction, affect, or affect reciprocity. | ||
Marci, Ham, Moran, and Orr (2007) | SCL | PsychoSoc | S | PS in SCL significantly and positively correlated with patient ratings of therapist empathy. Patients and therapists showed significantly more solidarity and positive regard when PS in SCL was high. |
Marci and Orr (2006) | SCL | Within | S | PS in SCL significantly higher between interviewer and patient during neutral than during emotionally distant condition. |
PsychoSoc | S | Patient ratings of interviewer empathy significantly higher in neutral than in emotionally distant condition. | ||
Messina et al. (2013) | SCL | Between | S | Significant differences in PS between the three groups (therapists, psychologists, and non-therapists) at Lag 0. PS in SCL with pseudo-patients was significantly higher with psychologists compared with therapists. At Lag 3, psychologists showed lower PS than therapists. |
PsychoSoc | S | PS in SCL between pseudo-patients and therapists was significantly correlated with empathy perceived by the pseudo-patients at 3- and 4-s lags. | ||
Järvelä, Kivikangas, Kätsyri, and Ravaja (2013) | SCR | General | S | PS in SCR significantly higher than zero among team members. |
Within | NS | PS in SCR not significantly different in cooperative/competitive conditions or with computer/non-computer players. | ||
PsychoSoc | NS | PS in SCR not significantly related to teammates’ emotional or behavioral self-report items. | ||
Kaplan, Burch, Bloom, and Edelberg (1963) | SCR | Between | S | PS in SCRs significantly more likely to occur in dyads who liked or disliked each other (as opposed to neutral rating). |
Robinson, Herman, and Kaplan (1982) | SCR | PsychoSoc | S | Therapist–client PS using SCR 1 (small but rapid skin conductance responses) and SCR 3 (large amplitude and short latency responses) significantly correlated with empathy. |
NS | Therapist–client PS using SCR 2 (responses of fairly large amplitude and long latency) and the composite measure (SCR 1, 2, and 3) of SCR types not significantly correlated with empathy. |
Source | Measure | Context | Finding | Results |
---|---|---|---|---|
Ferrer and Helm (2013) | Resp | General | S | PS observed between couples across all conditions, but substantial differences in synchrony parameters across couples. |
Within | NS | Coregulation not significantly different from zero at baseline. PS parameters did not differ by gender. | ||
S | PS parameters changed across conditions, but differed substantially across couples. | |||
PS increased from baseline through imitation task, indicating individuals adjusted their respiration more as a function of their partners’ breathing than their own. | ||||
PsychoSoc | S | Females’ PS in respiration during imitation task related to daily affect, suggesting females’ physiology adjusted to their male partners’ in this task similar to adjustments in response to their partners’ daily affect. | ||
Helm, Sbarra, and Ferrer (2012) | Resp | PsychoSoc | NS | Anxiety did not effect PS in respiration in any task. Length in the relationship not related to any cross-partner associations. |
S | Higher avoidance associated with reduced PS in respiration during resting and gazing tasks, but associated with increased PS for males during imitation task. Relationship satisfaction associated with different patterns of PS in respiration across tasks and genders. | |||
Muller and Lindenberger (2011) | Resp | Within | S | Magnitudes of PS in respiration were higher during singing than during rest. Directed positive PS where physiological changes in the conductor were followed by choir members. |
Chanel, Kivikangas, and Ravaja (2012) | Resp-Amp | General | S | PS in respiration amplitude significantly different from zero. |
Within | NS | No significant differences between cooperative and competitive play in respiration amplitude. | ||
Typology | S | PS positive for most participants, but negative for others. | ||
PsychoSoc | NS | No gaming experience self-report constructs significant covariates with PS in respiration amplitude. | ||
Chanel, Kivikangas, and Ravaja (2012) | HF-Resp-Amp | Within | S | PS in high-frequency respiration amplitude higher for competitive than for cooperative play. |
PsychoSoc | S | PS in high-frequency respiration amplitude positively correlated with social empathy self-evaluations. | ||
Bachrach, Fontbonne, Joufflineau, and Ulloa (2015) | RR | General | S | PS found between RR of audience members and dancers during a music-free, slow-paced, live dance performance. |
PsychoSoc | S | PS between RR of audience members and dancers was positively and significantly associated with audience members’ self-reported attention to their own and the dancers’ breathing. | ||
NS | PS between RR of audience members and dancers was not significantly associated with audience members’ appreciation for the performance. | |||
Codrons, Bernardi, Vandoni, and Bernardi (2014) | RR | Within | S | PS in 10-person groups was found during a still, silent baseline, and when swinging their arms while listening to music. |
NS | PS was not significant in 10-person groups when swinging their arms in silence or while listening to a metronome. | |||
Henning, Boucsein, and Gil (2001) | RR | Within | S | Teammates’ PS in RR a significant predictor of some performance measures. |
NS | Teammates’ PS in RR not a significant predictor of team behavioral coordination. | |||
McAssey, Helm, Hsieh, Sbarra, and Ferrer (2013) | RR | Within | S | PS in RR significantly increased from baseline to in-sync task for all four couples, but only for one couple during the gazing task. |
McFarland (2001) | RR | General | S | Friends’ PS in RR differed significantly in comparison with randomly selected simulated dyads. |
Chatel-Goldman, Congedo, Jutten, and Schwartz (2014) | RVT | General | NA | Significance of PS between couples’ respiration volume time was not tested. |
Within | NS | No significant differences in PS of respiration volume time when couples could or could not touch each other. | ||
PsychoSoc | NS | No significant correlations found between empathy scores and PS in respiration volume time. |
Source | Measure | Context | Finding | Results |
---|---|---|---|---|
Ebisch et al. (2012) | Temp | General | S | Significant correlations between facial temperature of mothers and their children. |
Manini et al. (2013) | Temp | General | S | Significant PS in facial temperature between women and children. |
Between | S | PS in mother–child group significantly higher during experiential condition than mother–other-child group. PS significantly higher and cross-correlation lags shorter between mothers and their own vs. other child dyads. | ||
Within | S | Significant differences in PS between mother–child dyads during neutral vs. experimental conditions. | ||
NS | No significant differences in PS across conditions in the mother–other-child group. | |||
Shearn, Spellman, Straley, Meirick, and Stryker (1999) | Blush | Between | S | PS in blushing between performers and friends, but not between friends and strangers or strangers and performers. |
Robinson, Herman, and Kaplan (1982) | FST | PsychoSoc | NS | PS in finger skin temperature of the counselor–client dyads not significantly correlated with empathy. |
Source | Measures | Context | Finding | Results |
---|---|---|---|---|
Levenson and Gottman (1983) | HR; PTT; SCL; ACTa | Within | S | PS detected when couples discussing conflict topics. |
NS | PS not detected when couples discussing neutral topics. | |||
PsychoSoc | Couples’ PS during arguments accounted for 60% of variance in marital satisfaction. | |||
Marci (2006) | SC; HR; RR; ACTa | Within | S | Arousal levels significantly different when commercials viewed in positive (i.e., during a highly rated show) and neutral (i.e., shown alone) contexts; both PS and arousal patterns changed when viewed during less positive context (i.e., a poorly rated show). |
NS | Patterns of PS not significantly different when commercials viewed in positive (i.e., during a highly rated show) and neutral (i.e., shown alone) contexts. | |||
Walker, Muth, Switzer, and Rosopa (2013) | LVET; PEP; RSA | Within | S | An index of PS significantly predicted team errors, but only accounted for a small proportion of variance in team error. |
References
Cite article
Cite article
Cite article
Download to reference manager
If you have citation software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice
Information, rights and permissions
Information
Published In

Keywords
Authors
Metrics and citations
Metrics
Journals metrics
This article was published in Personality and Social Psychology Review.
View All Journal MetricsArticle usage*
Total views and downloads: 8910
*Article usage tracking started in December 2016
Altmetric
See the impact this article is making through the number of times it’s been read, and the Altmetric Score.
Learn more about the Altmetric Scores
Articles citing this one
Receive email alerts when this article is cited
Web of Science: 302 view articles Opens in new tab
Crossref: 307
- Exploring differences in learners' learning processes across collaborative knowledge construction tasks of diverse complexity: A multiple analysis
- Emotional Foundations of Conspiracy Beliefs: From Individual Emotions to Emotional Sharing and Collective Emotion Regulation
- Physiological team dynamics explored: physiological synchrony in medical simulation training
- Multimodal interpersonal synchrony: Systematic review and meta-analysis
- Two ways to measure interpersonal synchrony in dance/movement therapy: Comparing accelerometer data with observational data
- Interpersonal synchronization as an objective measure of listening engagement
- Central (Hemodynamic) and Peripheral (Autonomic) Synergy During Persuasion Within a Shared Decision-Making Process
- Synchrony Across Brains
- Six weeks that changed the preterm infant brain: lessons learned from the Family Nurture Intervention randomized controlled trials
- Siblings’ similarity in neural responses to loss reflects mechanisms of familial transmission for depression
- View More
Figures and tables
Figures & Media
Tables
View Options
Access options
If you have access to journal content via a personal subscription, university, library, employer or society, select from the options below:
loading institutional access options
SPSP members can access this journal content using society membership credentials.
SPSP members can access this journal content using society membership credentials.
Alternatively, view purchase options below:
Purchase 24 hour online access to view and download content.
Access journal content via a DeepDyve subscription or find out more about this option.