Skip to main content

[]

Intended for healthcare professionals
Skip to main content
Restricted access
Other
First published April 1999

The Origins of Opinion of American Party Activists

Abstract

In this paper, I test two hypotheses about the origin of opinion of partisan activists. The first is a rational-choice thesis that states that due to electoral considerations, activists will adopt positions on issues that correspond to where they believe voters to stand on those issues. The competing hypothesis is based on the influence of contenders for the parties' presidential nomination. This hypothesis predicts that just as candidates play a role in activating segments of the party to participate in the nomination process, they also influence the views held by those activists. Using data from the 1988 and 1992 Convention Delegate Studies, I conduct tests of these hypotheses. The results point toward a candidate-motivation explanation for the origin of opinion of party activists. The implications of these findings are that the information about politics that flows to the mass citizenry is influenced by the candidates who choose to seek the party's nomination and, in the process, activate segments of the party that will then serve as the opinion leaders in society.

Get full access to this article

View all access and purchase options for this article.

I would like to thank Peter Esaiasson, John Geer, Cheryl L. Herrera and Carolyn Warner for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of this paper. Some data were made available by the Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research. Funding for the survey of delegates attending the 1992 conventions was provided by the National Science Foundation, grant SBR-9310507.
1 See Norris (1995) for an analysis of May's law in British party politics.
2 See Wattenberg (1990) for a discussion of the term `perceptual screen' with regard to the effects of partisan identification.
3 The 1988 and 1992 Convention Delegate Studies are the fourth and fifth surveys in a series of delegate surveys begun in 1972. The population for the studies are all delegates attending the Democratic and Republican national nominating conventions. See Appendices 1 and 2 for a list of the items used in the analysis from the two surveys.
4 As I suggested on p. 235, I modify the analysis in a couple of important ways from what typical Downsian analysis would suggest (May, 1979). First, the prediction by rational-choice models that the positions of activists will be most similar to those held by party voters as tested by a simple comparison of positions held by each strata is insufficient. Comparing the positions of activists and voters does not adequately address the underlying theory. The linchpin of this application of rational-choice theory is that the motivations of the party activists (i.e. electoral, candidate-focused) cause variability in the degree of proximity between them and voters. Given this premise, it may be misleading to only use the relative positions of activists vis-a-vis voters. Activists may misperceive the views of the voters, which may result in unintended differences between activists and voters. That is, activists may be unable to ascribe accurately the views of those partisans. Differences between the positions of activists and regular partisan voters, then, may not be due to the activists' lack of concern for matching the positions of voters but rather to inaccurate placement of regular partisans' positions on issues. Unless activists have perfect information about the spatial locations of their parties' voters, they cannot be expected to choose positions that are congruent with those of the rank-and-file, except by chance. We need, therefore, to control for that uncertainty about where voters are situated along the continuum of positions on issues. We can do that with data that provide not only the views of the activists on issues but their perceptions of what positions they believe partisans to hold on those same issues.
Second, I take into account the motivations of the activists when examining their views on politics. The adaptation of rational-choice models to the opinions held by partisans of various activity levels suggests that the motivations of the activists at those levels should matter. As I elaborate in the next section, each level of activist contains members who are motivated for more than one reason. That is, even though I look at activists from one of the higher rungs on the ladder, there are surely those who are motivated not so much by winning, but by other concerns.
5 A reference to wanting to get the party and its candidates to support particular policies was also frequently given. I associate that response with activists being interested in policies as well as candidates and winning elections. It does not, alone, indicate that issues are more important than candidates or winning, however. Among other reasons given are wanting to make business contacts, socializing with politicians, seeking careers in politics, and enjoying the fun and excitement of campaigns, but none of these reasons rivaled the frequency of the reasons listed in the tables.
6 I used the activists' perceptions of where their own partisan voters from their state were located. I also used the activists' perception of where their partisan voters across the nation were located and, in 1992, their perception of the national electorate's views. The results were similar to those presented here. In most cases the differences were larger than those shown here. In no instances were they smaller.
7 Appendix 2 summarizes the variables in the OLS equation that are used, and their accompanying labels.
8 Clearly, partisan affiliation and positions on issues explain much of the variance in activists' ideological self-designation. In the following analysis I control for the effects of partisan affiliation and issue positions, leaving, I expect, the two hypotheses as the sole explanatory variables. I do this by examining the activists from each party separately. In so doing, I control for partisan affiliation and positions on issues. Since the level of constraint found among these highly active partisans is quite high (Jennings, 1993), I can be fairly sure that the homogeneity of the party delegates will control for their positions on issues as well. I have therefore controlled for most of the variance to be found in activists' ideological self-placement. I should not be surprised, then, if the model does not perform well in term of R2 values since I am only explaining what is left after partisanship and issues are removed.
9 Why the fourfold difference? I suspect that the degree of affect toward a candidate has much to do with the magnitude of the candidate's effect. To check this notion, I compared the feeling thermometer scores for the two groups of candidate supporters. As anticipated, the average feeling thermometer score for Jesse Jackson by Jackson supporters was 10 points higher than the average score for Dukakis by Dukakis supporters (93 and 83 respectively).
10 Additional analyses were also conducted in which supporters of Jack Kemp were substituted for those of Robert Dole. This change resulted in near-identical coefficients across the board. Also, bivariate regressions were run as a check on the multiplicative variables included in the models presented. Controls were made for those activists who preferred candidates and/or were motivated to win elections. In all cases, the results supported those reported here.
11 I also conducted aggregate-level analysis and obtained further support for the candidate-motivation hypothesis.

References

Aldrich, John H. (1983) `A Downsian Spatial Model with Party Activists', American Political Science Review 77: 974-990.
Beck, Paul Allen and M. Kent Jennings (1979) `Political Periods and Political Participation', American Political Science Review 73: 737-750.
Beck, Paul Allen and M. Kent Jennings (1984) `Updating Political Periods and Political Participation', American Political Science Review 78: 198-201.
Berelson, Bernard R., Paul F. Lazarsfeld and William N. McPhee (1954) Voting. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Campbell, Angus, Philip E. Converse, Warren E. Miller and Donald E. Stokes (1960) The American Voter. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Carmines, Edward G. and James A. Stimson (1989) Issue Evolution, Race and the Transformation of American Politics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Ceasar, James W. (1982) Reforming the Reforms. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger Publishing Co.
Converse, Philip E. (1964) `The Nature of Belief Systems in Mass Publics', in David E. Apter (ed.) Ideology and Discontent. New York: Free Press.
Downs, Anthony (1957) An Economic Theory of Democracy. New York: Harper and Row.
Finkel, Steven E. And Gregory Trevor (1986) `Reassessing Ideological Bias in Campaign Participation', Political Behavior 8: 374-390.
Friedrich, Robert J. (1982) `In Defense of Multiplicative Terms in Multiple Regression Equations', American Journal of Political Science 26: 797-833.
Herrera, Richard (1995) `The Crosswinds of Change: Sources of Change in the Democratic and Republican Parties', Political Research Quarterly 48: 291-312.
Herrera, Richard and Melanie K. Taylor (1994) `The Structure of Opinion in American Political Parties', Political Studies 42: 676-689.
Jennings, M. Kent (1993) `Ideology among Mass Publics and Political Elites', Public Opinion Quarterly 56: 419-441.
Key, V. O. (1964) Politics, Parties and Pressure Groups, 5th edn. New York: Thomas Y. Crowell Company.
May, John D. (1973) `Opinion Structure of Political Parties: The Special Law of Curvilinear Disparity', Political Studies 21: 135-151.
McClosky, Herbert and John Zaller (1984) The American Ethos. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Norris, Pippa (1995) `May's Law of Curvilinear Disparity Revisited: Leaders, Officers, Members and Voters in British Political Parties', Party Politics 1: 29-47.
Polsby, Nelson W. (1983) Consequences of Party Reform. New York: Oxford University Press.
Polsby, Nelson and Aaron Wildavsky (1991) Presidential Elections, 8th edn. New York: Free Press.
Wattenberg, Martin P. (1990) The Decline of American Political Parties, 1952-88. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wattenberg, Martin P. (1991) The Rise of Candidate-Centered Politics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Zaller, John R. (1992) The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Cite article

Cite article

Cite article

OR

Download to reference manager

If you have citation software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice

Share options

Share

Share this article

Share with email
Email Article Link
Share on social media

Share access to this article

Sharing links are not relevant where the article is open access and not available if you do not have a subscription.

For more information view the Sage Journals article sharing page.

Information, rights and permissions

Information

Published In

Article first published: April 1999
Issue published: April 1999

Keywords

  1. American party activists
  2. candidate-centered politics
  3. opinion structure
  4. rational choice

Rights and permissions

Request permissions for this article.

Authors

Affiliations

Metrics and citations

Metrics

Journals metrics

This article was published in Party Politics.

View All Journal Metrics

Article usage*

Total views and downloads: 40

*Article usage tracking started in December 2016


Altmetric

See the impact this article is making through the number of times it’s been read, and the Altmetric Score.
Learn more about the Altmetric Scores



Articles citing this one

Receive email alerts when this article is cited

Web of Science: 1 view articles Opens in new tab

Crossref: 4

  1. 정치환경의 변화와 정당개혁 - 미국 정당들의 개혁과 변화로부터의 교훈
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle Scholar
  2. The Influence of Tone, Target, and Issue Ownership on Political Advertising Effects in Primary Versus General Elections
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle Scholar
  3. Research, Revisionists and the Radical Right
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle Scholar
  4. THE RISE AND FAULTS OF THE INTERNALIST PERSPECTIVE IN EXTREME RIGHT STUDIES
    Go to citationCrossrefGoogle Scholar

Figures and tables

Figures & Media

Tables

View Options

Access options

If you have access to journal content via a personal subscription, university, library, employer or society, select from the options below:


Alternatively, view purchase options below:

Purchase 24 hour online access to view and download content.

Access journal content via a DeepDyve subscription or find out more about this option.

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB